Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex
暂无分享,去创建一个
Marko Nardini | Tessa M. Dekker | Hiroshi Ban | Andrew E. Welchman | Martin I. Sereno | Bauke van der Velde | M. Sereno | M. Nardini | A. Welchman | T. Dekker | Hiroshi Ban | Bauke van der Velde | H. Ban
[1] Benjamin A. Rowland,et al. Incorporating Cross-Modal Statistics in the Development and Maintenance of Multisensory Integration , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[2] Current Biology , 2012, Current Biology.
[3] A. Pouget,et al. Not Noisy, Just Wrong: The Role of Suboptimal Inference in Behavioral Variability , 2012, Neuron.
[4] Jonathan D. Power,et al. Functional Brain Networks Develop from a “Local to Distributed” Organization , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[5] I. Howard. Perceiving in DepthVolume 3 Other Mechanisms of Depth Perception , 2012 .
[6] Chih-Jen Lin,et al. LIBSVM: A library for support vector machines , 2011, TIST.
[7] M. Landy,et al. Weighted linear cue combination with possibly correlated error , 2003, Vision Research.
[8] M. Nardini,et al. Fusion of visual cues is not mandatory in children , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[9] Cristina Montomoli,et al. The Development of Visual Object Recognition in School-Age Children , 2007, Developmental neuropsychology.
[10] Lynne Kiorpes,et al. Visual development in primates: Neural mechanisms and critical periods , 2015, Developmental neurobiology.
[11] Benjamin A Rowland,et al. Initiating the Development of Multisensory Integration by Manipulating Sensory Experience , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] D. Heeger,et al. Two Retinotopic Visual Areas in Human Lateral Occipital Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] Thomas F. Nugent,et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[14] Martin I. Sereno,et al. Dorsal and Ventral Stream Activation and Object Recognition Performance in School-age Children , 2010 .
[15] Pete R. Jones,et al. Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation , 2008, Current Biology.
[16] G. DeAngelis,et al. Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[17] David C. Burr,et al. Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information , 2008, Current Biology.
[18] Jennifer M. D. Yoon,et al. Striking deficiency in top-down perceptual reorganization of two-tone images in preschool children , 2007, 2007 IEEE 6th International Conference on Development and Learning.
[19] A. Norcia,et al. Spatial frequency sweep VEP: Visual acuity during the first year of life , 1985, Vision Research.
[20] M. Nardini,et al. When vision is not an option: children’s integration of auditory and haptic information is suboptimal , 2014, Developmental science.
[21] A. Clark. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[22] Karl J. Friston. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[23] B. Rogers,et al. Similarities between motion parallax and stereopsis in human depth perception , 1982, Vision Research.
[24] S. Petersen,et al. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[25] M. Lazarides. Perceiving depth , 1991, Nature.
[26] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[27] Janette Atkinson,et al. Infants' sensitivity to motion and temporal change. , 2009, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.
[28] Bruce C. Hansen,et al. The developing visual system is not optimally sensitive to the spatial statistics of natural images , 2012, Vision Research.
[29] James M. Hillis,et al. Slant from texture and disparity cues: optimal cue combination. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[30] Rainer Goebel,et al. Information-based functional brain mapping. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[31] A. Norcia,et al. Neural noise limitations on infant visual sensitivity , 1998, Nature.
[32] Hiroshi Ban,et al. The integration of motion and disparity cues to depth in dorsal visual cortex , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] Emily Buss,et al. Psychometric functions for pure tone intensity discrimination: slope differences in school-aged children and adults. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[34] Alex R. Wade,et al. Visual areas and spatial summation in human visual cortex , 2001, Vision Research.
[35] R. Malach,et al. Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[36] G. Orban,et al. The kinetic occipital region in human visual cortex. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.
[37] Hiroshi Ban,et al. Integration of texture and disparity cues to surface slant in dorsal visual cortex. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[38] Barry E Stein,et al. Visual Experience Is Necessary for the Development of Multisensory Integration , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[39] John M. Findlay,et al. The area of spatial integration for initial horizontal disparity vergence , 1998, Vision Research.
[40] A. Dale,et al. High‐resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface , 1999, Human brain mapping.
[41] Hiroshi Ban,et al. Perceptual Integration for Qualitatively Different 3-D Cues in the Human Brain , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.