Human Path Integration by Optic Flow

Path integration or 'dead reckoning' is the ability to keep track of relative position using self-motion signals that convey information about speed and direction of movement. Most animal species, including humans, exhibit some degree of path integration capability and neurophysiological studies have demonstrated that self-motion signals are sufficient to update internal representations of both position and orientation. In the present study, human subjects were required to monitor their position or orientation on the basis of unstructured optic flowfields. Trials were conducted at different speeds to examine the accuracy of path integration and rates of random error accumulation, and at two different head azimuths to prevent a confounding strategy of position updating based primarily on tracking changes in the angular declination of distant landmarks with respect to the horizon. Participants integrated the speed of visual motion to update accurately a representation of their position and orientation within the environment. Consistent with the characteristics of real-world path integration, errors accumulated linearly with the magnitude of position and orientation estimation. We conclude that coherent optic flowfields provide a sufficient basis for humans to keep track of their position and orientation relative to remembered landmarks.

[1]  A Berthoz,et al.  Spatial memory of body linear displacement: what is being stored? , 1995, Science.

[2]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Mechanismen der Orientierung ohne richtende Außenreize , 1973 .

[3]  M. Mittelstaedt Influence of centrifugal force on angular velocity estimation. , 1995, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[4]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[5]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Reinhart Jürgens,et al.  Circular vection during voluntary suppression of optokinetic reflex , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in Gerbils and Humans , 1991 .

[8]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[9]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  C. Darwin Origin of Certain Instincts , 1873, Nature.

[11]  W. Becker,et al.  Vestibular, optokinetic, and cognitive contribution to the guidance of passive self-rotation toward instructed targets , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[13]  I. Israël,et al.  Self-rotation estimate about the vertical axis. , 1995, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[14]  K M Gothard,et al.  Dynamics of Mismatch Correction in the Hippocampal Ensemble Code for Space: Interaction between Path Integration and Environmental Cues , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Discrimination of travel distances from ‘situated’ optic flow , 2003, Vision Research.

[16]  M. Gresty,et al.  Self‐Controlled Reorienting Movements in Response to Rotational Displacements in Normal Subjects and Patients with Labyrinthine Disease , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual Homing Is Possible Without Landmarks: A Path Integration Study in Virtual Reality , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[18]  A. Etienne,et al.  Limitations in the Assessment of Path Dependent Information , 1988 .

[19]  Kevin G. F. Thomas,et al.  Place Learning in Virtual Space I: Acquisition, Overshadowing, and Transfer , 1997 .

[20]  E. Rolls,et al.  Head direction cells in the primate pre‐subiculum , 1999, Hippocampus.

[21]  W E Skaggs,et al.  Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: the hippocampus as a path integration system. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[22]  P Péruch,et al.  Homing in Virtual Environments: Effects of Field of View and Path Layout , 1997, Perception.

[23]  William H Warren,et al.  Path Integration from Optic Flow and Body Senses in a Homing Task , 2002, Perception.

[24]  F. Bremmer,et al.  The use of optical velocities for distance discrimination and reproduction during visually simulated self motion , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon , 2001, Nature.

[26]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  The Head-neck sensory motor system , 1992 .

[27]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Seeking one’s heading through eye movements , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  H Mittelstaedt,et al.  Interaction of eye-, head-, and trunk-bound information in spatial perception and control. , 1997, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[29]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[30]  R. Ivry,et al.  Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  W. Bles,et al.  Somatosensory compensation for loss of labyrinthine function. , 1984, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[32]  S Krafczyk,et al.  Interaction between perceived self-motion and object-motion impairs vehicle guidance. , 1984, Science.

[33]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by Path Integration , 1982 .

[34]  H. Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by path integration in a mammal , 1980, Naturwissenschaften.

[35]  Charles J. Duffy,et al.  Cortical Neurons Encoding Path and Place: Where You Go Is Where You Are , 2002, Science.

[36]  Hanspeter A Mallot,et al.  Route Navigating without Place Recognition: What is Recognised in Recognition-Triggered Responses? , 2000, Perception.

[37]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Estimation of passive horizontal linear whole-body displacement in humans. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection) basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  James A. Crowell,et al.  The perception of heading during eye movements , 1992, Nature.

[40]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[41]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Spatial memory in large scale movements: Efficiency and limitation of the egocentric coding process , 1990 .

[43]  B. Heller Circular Statistics in Biology, Edward Batschelet. Academic Press, London & New York (1981), 371, Price $69.50 , 1983 .

[44]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.

[45]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  The Effects of Proprioceptive and Visual Feedback on Geographical Orientation in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[46]  Michael Jenkin,et al.  Humans can use optic flow to estimate distance of travel , 2001, Vision Research.