Attention and time constraints in perceptual-motor learning and performance: Instruction, analogy, and skill level

We sought to gain more insight into the effects of attention focus and time constraints on skill learning and performance in novices and experts by means of two complementary experiments using a table tennis paradigm. Experiment 1 showed that skill-focus conditions and slowed ball frequency disrupted the accuracy of experts, but dual-task conditions and speeded ball frequency did not. For novices, only speeded ball frequency disrupted accuracy. In Experiment 2, we extended these findings by instructing novices either explicitly or by analogy (implicit motor learning technique). Explicitly instructed novices were less accurate in skill-focused and dual-task conditions than in single-task conditions. Following analogy instruction novices were less accurate in the skill-focused condition, but maintained accuracy under dual-task conditions. Participants in both conditions retained accuracy when ball frequency was slowed, but lost accuracy when ball frequency was speeded, suggesting that not attention, but motor dexterity, was inadequate under high temporal constraints.

[1]  Kelly J. Ashford,et al.  Attentional Focus, Dispositional Reinvestment, and Skilled Motor Performance under Pressure , 2006 .

[2]  Rich S. W. Masters,et al.  Motor Performance as a Function of Audience Affability and Metaknowledge , 2003 .

[3]  A H FUCHS The progression-regression hypotheses in perceptual-motor skill learning. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  Didier Delignières,et al.  Influence of a stressing constraint on stiffness and damping functions of a ski simulator's platform motion , 2004, Journal of sports sciences.

[5]  A Mark Williams,et al.  Online Attentional-Focus Manipulations in a Soccer-Dribbling Task: Implications for the Proceduralization of Motor Skills , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  J. Maxwell,et al.  Passing thoughts on the evolutionary stability of implicit motor behaviour: Performance retention under physiological fatigue , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[7]  J. Maxwell,et al.  The relationship between initial errorless learning conditions and subsequent performance. , 2005, Human movement science.

[8]  Explicit rules and direction of attention in table-tennis forehand performance , 2007 .

[9]  Lew Hardy,et al.  Implicit and explicit control of motor actions: revisiting some early evidence. , 2007, British journal of psychology.

[10]  R. Masters,et al.  Implicit motor learning, reinvestment and movement disruption : What you don’t know won’t hurt you , 2004 .

[11]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  E. R. Crossman A THEORY OF THE ACQUISITION OF SPEED-SKILL∗ , 1959 .

[13]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Effects of focus of attention depend on golfers' skill , 2003, Journal of sports sciences.

[14]  Justin T. Maxwell,et al.  The implicit benefit of learning without errors , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[15]  R. Masters,et al.  Benefits of an external focus of attention: Common coding or conscious processing? , 2006, Journal of sports sciences.

[16]  N. A. Bernstein Dexterity and Its Development , 1996 .

[17]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Rules of the Mind , 1993 .

[18]  Justin T. Maxwell,et al.  From novice to no know-how: A longitudinal study of implicit motor learning , 2000, Journal of sports sciences.

[19]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Putting in the mind versus putting on the green: Expertise, performance time, and the linking of imagery and action , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  A. M. Gentile,et al.  Movement Science: Implicit and Explicit Processes during Acquisition of Functional Skills , 1998 .

[21]  R. Masters Theoretical aspects of implicit learning in sport. , 2000 .

[22]  Sher ry Folsom-Meek,et al.  Human Performance , 2020, Nature.

[23]  Cognitive demands of error processing associated with preparation and execution of a motor skill , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[24]  R. Gray,et al.  Effects of focus of attention on baseball batting performance in players of differing skill levels. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[25]  R. Masters,et al.  Analogy learning: A means to implicit motor learning , 2001, Journal of sports sciences.

[26]  Larry Hodges Table Tennis: Steps to Success , 1993 .

[27]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Proportional and nonproportional transfer of movement sequences , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  B. P. Lewis,et al.  Thinking about Choking? Attentional Processes and Paradoxical Performance , 1997, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[29]  T. Carr,et al.  “Don’t Miss!” The Debilitating Effects of Suppressive Imagery on Golf Putting Performance , 2001 .

[30]  F. Kottke From reflex to skill: the training of coordination. , 1980, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[31]  P. Beek,et al.  Rule formation and table tennis performance following explicit and analogy learning over 10,000 repetitions. , 2008 .

[32]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Expertise, attention, and memory in sensorimotor skill execution: Impact of novel task constraints on dual-task performance and episodic memory , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[33]  A. E. Schulman,et al.  Functional coupling and regional activation of human cortical motor areas during simple, internally paced and externally paced finger movements. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Skill in Sport , 2007 .

[35]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Toward a theory of implicit learning in the perceptual-motor domain. , 2000 .

[36]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  Rich S. W. Masters,et al.  The theory of reinvestment , 2008 .

[38]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  J. Maxwell,et al.  Analogy learning and the performance of motor skills under pressure. , 2009, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[40]  R. Masters Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure , 1992 .

[41]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[42]  C. Shea,et al.  Effector Independence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[43]  L. Hardy,et al.  Knowledge and conscious control of motor actions under stress. , 1996, British journal of psychology.

[44]  R. Baumeister Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  L. Cosmides From : The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1995 .

[46]  R. Masters,et al.  Self-Focused Attention and Performance Failure under Psychological Stress. , 2002, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[47]  R. Gray Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: expertise differences, choking, and slumps. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[48]  Rich S. W. Masters,et al.  ‘Reinvestment’: A dimension of personality implicated in skill breakdown under pressure , 1993 .

[49]  M Hallett,et al.  Self-paced versus metronome-paced finger movements. A positron emission tomography study. , 1997, Journal of neuroimaging : official journal of the American Society of Neuroimaging.

[50]  R S W Masters,et al.  Implicit Motor Learning and Complex Decision Making in Time-Constrained Environments , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.

[51]  D. Rosenbaum On choosing between movement sequences: comments on Rose (1988). , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  Koedijker J M,et al.  21 Chapter 2 Explicit rules and direction of attention in learning and performing the table tennis forehand , 2010 .

[53]  J. Maxwell,et al.  Analogy versus explicit learning of a modified basketball shooting task: Performance and kinematic outcomes , 2009, Journal of sports sciences.

[54]  A. Williams,et al.  Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice , 2005, Sport & Exercise Psychology Review.

[55]  David A. Caulton,et al.  On the Modularity of Sequence Representation , 1995 .

[56]  R. Masters,et al.  The influence of analogy learning on decision-making in table tennis: Evidence from behavioural data , 2006 .

[57]  C. Janelle,et al.  Implicit overcompensation: The influence of negative self-instructions on performance of a self-paced motor task , 2008, Journal of sports sciences.

[58]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The Capacity for Generating Information by Randomization , 1966, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[59]  K. Doya,et al.  Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[60]  J. Dimmock,et al.  Choking under pressure in sensorimotor skills: Conscious processing or depleted attentional resources? , 2008 .

[61]  J P Maxwell,et al.  Implicit motor learning in surgery: implications for multi-tasking. , 2008, Surgery.

[62]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. , 1996, Psychological review.

[63]  Michael Hoerger,et al.  When does haste make waste? Speed-accuracy tradeoff, skill level, and the tools of the trade. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[64]  J. Maxwell,et al.  Stable implicit motor processes despite aerobic locomotor fatigue , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[65]  B. Abernethy Dual-task methodology and motor skills research: Some applications and methodological constraints , 1988 .

[66]  C. Shea,et al.  The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.