Vision Following Extended Congenital Blindness

Animal studies suggest that early visual deprivation can cause permanent functional blindness. However, few human data on this issue exist. Given enough time for recovery, can a person gain visual skills after several years of congenital blindness? In India, we recently had an unusual opportunity to work with an individual whose case history sheds light on this question. S.R.D. was born blind, and remained so until age 12. She then underwent surgery for the removal of dense congenital cataracts. We evaluated her performance on an extensive battery of visual tasks 20 years after surgery. We found that although S.R.D.'s acuity is compromised, she is proficient on mid- and high-level visual tasks. These results suggest that the human brain retains an impressive capacity for visual learning well into late childhood. They have implications for current conceptions of cortical plasticity and provide an argument for treating congenital blindness even in older children.

[1]  A. Posner Learning to see. , 1955, Eye, ear, nose & throat monthly.

[2]  M. V. Senden Space and sight : the perception of space and shape in the congenitally blind before and after operation , 1960 .

[3]  J. G. Wallace,et al.  Recovery from early blindness : a case study , 1963 .

[4]  D. Hubel,et al.  Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  R. Held,et al.  Development and segmentation of visually controlled movement by selective exposure during rearing. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  A. Riesen Sight Restoration After Long-Term Blindness: The Problems and Behavior Patterns of Visual Rehabilitation , 1972 .

[7]  E. Warrington,et al.  Lasting Effects of Early Blindness a Case Study , 1974, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  R Held,et al.  Comparison of visually guided reaching in normal and deprived infant monkeys. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  S. Carey,et al.  Developmental changes in the representation of faces. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  D. Hubel,et al.  Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. , 1977, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  D. Hubel,et al.  The development of ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[12]  S. Carlson,et al.  VISUAL REHABILITATION AFTER LONG LASTING EARLY BLINDNESS , 1983, Acta ophthalmologica.

[13]  A. Raninen,et al.  Persistent behavioural blindness after early visual deprivation and active visual rehabilitation: a case report. , 1986, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[14]  8. Learning how to see , 1998 .

[15]  David K. A. Barnes,et al.  correction: Early visual experience and face processing , 2001, Nature.

[16]  D. Maurer,et al.  Neuroperception: Early visual experience and face processing , 2001, Nature.

[17]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  Long-term deprivation affects visual perception and cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  M. Fahle To see and not to see , 2004, Nature.

[19]  Catherine J. Mondloch,et al.  Missing sights: consequences for visual cognitive development , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  A. Mandavilli Visual neuroscience: Look and learn , 2006, Nature.