Emotion, rationality, and social identity: a theoretical–methodological proposal for a cognitive approach

Humberto Maturana's biology of cognition and the studies of neuroscientist Antonio Damasio have proposed theories that indicate the role of emotion in cognitive processes. Emotion lies at the basis of any behavior. An emotional transition defines a transition from one domain of actions to the other, while emotions shared in human interaction define collective domains of action and thus also social identity. In this case, they may give rise to operational coherences within the same domain of action which is recognized as "rationality". The transition from one operational coherence to another within the same rationality does not therefore entail an emotional transition. This article uses this theoretical framework to propose an interpretation of the phenomenon of social identity and distinguishes social identities based only on emotions from those related to operational coherence. We call the former type of social identity "ontological", while the latter is referred to as "formal". To empirically prove the theoretical proposal of the article, the concept of metacontrast found in social self-categorization theory and its quantification elaborated by Alberto Voci are used. According to the social self-categorization theory, the greater the distance between two groups' visions of the world, the greater the metacontrast between them. Using our model, metacontrast must thus be greater for ontological social identity and less for formal social identity. Our hypothesis was confirmed by the data. This outcome allows huge applications of the methodology proposed, as exemplified in the final considerations.

[1]  S. Karau,et al.  PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN , 1999 .

[2]  F. Tönnies,et al.  Community and Society , 1988 .

[3]  Marie J. Lachance,et al.  Adolescents’ brand sensitivity in apparel: influence of three socialization agents , 2003 .

[4]  M. Brewer The Social Self: On Being the Same and Different at the Same Time , 1991 .

[5]  J. Baudrillard The Consumer Society: Myths and Structures , 2016 .

[6]  John C. Turner,et al.  From personal pictures in the head to collective tools in the world: how shared stereotypes allow groups to represent and change social reality , 2002 .

[7]  John C. Turner,et al.  Self and Collective: Cognition and Social Context , 1994 .

[8]  N. K. Hayles Cognition Everywhere: The Rise of the Cognitive Nonconscious and the Costs of Consciousness , 2014 .

[9]  E. Durkheim FROM THE DIVISION OF LABOR IN SOCIETY , 2019, The New Economic Sociology.

[10]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Approximate Power and Sample Size Determination for Common one-Sample and two-Sample Hypothesis Tests , 1970 .

[11]  Paolo Totaro,et al.  Biological Recursion and Digital Systems: Conceptual Tools for Analysing Man-Machine Interaction , 2020 .

[12]  H. Maturana Reality: The Search for Objectivity or the Quest for a Compelling Argument , 1988 .

[13]  A. Damasio Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. avon books , 1994 .

[14]  C. Cooley Social Organization: A Study of the Larger Mind , 1962 .

[15]  Jennifer Edson Escalas,et al.  Self‐Construal, Reference Groups, and Brand Meaning , 2005 .

[16]  James E. Burroughs,et al.  Family structure, materialism, and compulsive consumption , 1997 .

[17]  Kara Chan,et al.  Materialism and Social Comparison among Adolescents , 2007 .

[18]  T. Abel,et al.  Mind, Self, and Society , 1934 .

[19]  H. Maturana Biology of Language: The Epistemology of Reality , 2014 .

[20]  P. Totaro,et al.  The duality of social self-categorization in consumption , 2019 .

[21]  James A. Roberts,et al.  Interpersonal influence and adolescent materialism and compulsive buying , 2008 .

[22]  Marshall W. Meyer,et al.  Bureaucracy in Modern Society , 1971 .

[23]  A. Giddens The consequences of modernity , 1990 .

[24]  Mark L. Johnson The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness (review) , 2001 .

[25]  Soyeon Shim,et al.  Profiling Adolescent Consumer Decision-Making Styles: Effects of Socialization Agents and Social-Structural Variables , 1997 .

[26]  H. Maturana,et al.  The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding , 2007 .

[27]  R. Merton Social Theory and Social Structure , 1958 .

[28]  Terry L. Childers,et al.  The Influence of Familial and Peer-Based Reference Groups on Consumer Decisions , 1992 .

[29]  G. Simmel Fashion , 1957, American Journal of Sociology.

[30]  S. Lash Power after Hegemony , 2007 .

[31]  A. Damasio Descartes’ Error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York (Grosset/Putnam) 1994. , 1994 .

[32]  M. Brewer,et al.  In-Group Identification as a Function of Depersonalization, Distinctiveness, and Status , 1993 .

[33]  Humberto R. Maturana,et al.  Science and Daily Life: The Ontology of Scientific Explanations , 1990 .

[34]  C. Heath,et al.  Where Consumers Diverge from Others: Identity Signaling and Product Domains , 2007 .

[35]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[36]  John Cheney-Lippold,et al.  A New Algorithmic Identity , 2011 .

[37]  M. Hogg,et al.  Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. , 1989 .

[38]  H. Maturana,et al.  Autopoiesis and Cognition , 1980 .

[39]  Tal Katz-Navon,et al.  Being the same and different: A model explaining new product adoption , 2008 .

[40]  A. Voci Relevance of social categories, depersonalization and group processes: two field tests of self-categorization theory , 2006 .