On the cognitive aspects of the joke

Abstract The present study of the mechanism of humor concentrates on the notion of surprise in semantic jokes. It explicates the ‘grammar’ of the joke and attempts to differentiate it from both the standard informative text on the one hand, and the ‘witty’ texts on the other. Both the ‘grammar’ of standard informative texts and the ‘grammar’ of the joke derive from principles regulating concept formation. In previous works (Giora 1985, 1988), I showed that categorial organization as delineated by Rosch (1973) and Rosch and Mervis (1975) for example, is applicable to the structure of informative texts. In this study I consider the structure of the joke along the same lines. Given the conditions for text well-formedness (the ‘Relevance Requirement’ (e.g., Grice 1975, Giora 1985) and the ‘Graded Informativeness Requirement’ (Giora 1988, following Grice 1975, Shannon 1951, Attneave 1959)), a joke is well-formed if and only if it obeys the ‘Relevance Requirement’, violates the ‘Graded Informativeness Requirement’ (the ‘Marked Informativeness Requirement’) and forces the reader to cancel the immediate unmarked interpretation of the text and replace it with a marked interpretation. The notion of markedness relies on categorial internal structuring which differentiates between the cognitive status of the prototype (the unmarked member) and the marginal status of the marked member.

[1]  James R. Lackner,et al.  Resolving ambiguity: Effects of biasing context in the unattended ear , 1972 .

[2]  T. Kuhlman A study of salience and motivational theories of humor , 1985 .

[3]  Operationalization of Incongruity in Humour Research: A Critique and Suggestions , 1977 .

[4]  J. Morreall The Philosophy of laughter and humor , 1987 .

[5]  S. Goldberg,et al.  The acquisition and violation of expectancy: an experimental paradigm. , 1969, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  Judged humor, arousal, and the relief theory. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Semantic mechanisms of humor , 1988 .

[8]  M. Lewis,et al.  The magnitude of the orienting response in children as a function of changes in color and contour , 1969 .

[9]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words , 1980, Biological Psychology.

[10]  Arthur C. Graesser,et al.  Wit and humor in discourse processing , 1988 .

[11]  J. Orbach,et al.  Reversibility of the Necker Cube: I. An Examination of the Concept of “Satiation of Orientation” , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.

[12]  W. R. Garner Applications of Information Theory to Psychology , 1959 .

[13]  E. Rosch ON THE INTERNAL STRUCTURE OF PERCEPTUAL AND SEMANTIC CATEGORIES1 , 1973 .

[14]  Victor Raskin,et al.  Semantic mechanisms of humor , 1984 .

[15]  A. Koestler The Act of Creation , 1964 .

[16]  RACHEL GIORA,et al.  A TEXT-BASED ANALYSIS OF NON-NARRATIVE TEXTS , 1985 .

[17]  Arthur C. Graesser,et al.  What are the cognitive and conceptual components of humorous text , 1989 .

[18]  Jerry Suls,et al.  Cognitive and Disparagement Theories of Humour: A Theoretical and Empirical Synthesis , 1977 .

[19]  J. Mandler Stories, Scripts, and Scenes: Aspects of Schema Theory , 1984 .

[20]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Categories and concepts , 1984 .

[22]  Rated funniness and dissimilarity of figures Divergence from expectancy , 1975 .

[23]  M. Kilwein,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories revisited : a replication with sighted and blind college students / , 1993 .

[24]  Michael B. Eisenberg,et al.  On Defining Relevance , 1991 .

[25]  Hugh John McNabb,et al.  THE INFECTION OF CHICKEN-POX , 1911 .

[26]  Jerry Suls,et al.  A two-stage model for the appreciation of jokes and cartoons , 1972 .

[27]  J. Morreall Taking Laughter Seriously , 1984 .

[28]  J. Fodor Psychology and Language. , 1970 .

[29]  Claude E. Shannon,et al.  Prediction and Entropy of Printed English , 1951 .

[30]  H. Foot,et al.  It's a Funny Thing, Humour , 1980 .

[31]  Paul E. McGhee,et al.  Handbook of humor research , 1983 .

[32]  J. Suls Chapter 4 – A Two-Stage Model for the Appreciation of Jokes and Cartoons: An Information-Processing Analysis , 1972 .

[33]  S. Kreitler,et al.  How to kill jokes cognitively? The meaning structure of jokes , 1988 .

[34]  R. Giora On the so-called evaluative material in informative text , 1990 .

[35]  Anthony J. Sanford,et al.  Cognition and Cognitive Psychology , 1985 .

[36]  A. Tversky Features of Similarity , 1977 .

[37]  H. Grice Logic and conversation , 1975 .

[38]  F. Attneave Multistability in perception. , 1971, Scientific American.

[39]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  An Investigation of a Satiation Process in the Reversible Perspective of Revolving Skeletal Shapes , 1961 .

[40]  J. Suls Cognitive Processes in Humor Appreciation , 1983 .

[41]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials to grammatical errors and semantic anomalies , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[42]  Rachel Giora,et al.  On the informativeness requirement , 1988 .

[43]  R. Leeper A Study of a Neglected Portion of the Field of Learning—the Development of Sensory Organization , 1935 .

[44]  T. Shultz A Cognitive-Developmental Analysis of Humour , 1976 .

[45]  D. Zillmann,et al.  Directionality of transitory dominance as a communication variable affecting humor appreciation. , 1972, Journal of personality and social psychology.