Are preschoolers who spontaneously create patterns better in mathematics?

BACKGROUND Early patterning competence has recently been identified as an important precursor of mathematical development. Whereas the focus of this research has been on children's ability regarding repeating patterns, children might also differ in their spontaneous attention to patterns. AIMS The present study aimed to explore 4- to 5-year olds' Spontaneous Focusing On Patterns (SFOP) and its association with their patterning and mathematical ability. SAMPLE Participants were 378 children (Mage  = 4 years 10 months; 191 boys) from 17 preschools. METHODS Spontaneous Focusing On Patterns was measured with a construction task in which children had to build a tower with 15 blocks of three different colours. The constructions of the children were grouped into three categories (i.e., pattern, random, and sorting). We additionally administered tasks assessing their patterning ability, mathematical ability, spatial ability, and visuospatial working memory. RESULTS When building a tower, 37% of the preschoolers spontaneously created a pattern, 49% made a random construction, and 14% sorted the blocks per colour. Preschoolers who spontaneously created a pattern had better patterning and mathematical ability than children in the random group. Group differences in patterning ability and spatial skills accounted for the difference in mathematical ability. CONCLUSIONS The current data suggest that children's spontaneous attention to patterns is an important component of their mathematical ability. Children's spontaneous pattern constructions may provide opportunities to discuss and practice patterns in preschool settings or at home, but more research is required to further analyse the role of SFOP in early mathematical development.

[1]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Whole Number Thinking, Learning and Development: Neuro-cognitive, Cognitive and Developmental Approaches , 2018 .

[2]  Emily R. Fyfe,et al.  Early Math Trajectories: Low-Income Children's Mathematics Knowledge From Ages 4 to 11. , 2017, Child development.

[3]  Miriam Lüken,et al.  Young Children’s Structure Sense , 2012 .

[4]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Spontaneous focusing on numerosity: A review of recent research , 2016 .

[5]  L. Steen The Science of Patterns , 1988, Science.

[6]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Spontaneous focusing on Arabic number symbols and its association with early mathematical competencies , 2019, Early Childhood Research Quarterly.

[7]  David J. Purpura,et al.  Informal Numeracy Skills , 2013 .

[8]  E. Lehtinen,et al.  Preschool Children’s Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity, Subitizing, and Counting Skills as Predictors of Their Mathematical Performance Seven Years Later at School , 2015 .

[9]  Paul Andrews,et al.  Identifying Opportunities for Grade One Children to Acquire Foundational Number Sense: Developing a Framework for Cross Cultural Classroom Analyses , 2015 .

[10]  D. Ansari,et al.  Relations between numerical, spatial, and executive function skills and mathematics achievement: A latent-variable approach , 2019, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Young Children’s Patterning Competencies and Mathematical Development: A Review , 2019, Mathematical Learning and Cognition in Early Childhood.

[12]  William D. McKillip “Patterns”—a mathematics unit for three- and four-year-olds , 1970 .

[13]  T. Cooper,et al.  Repeating Patterns and Multiplicative Thinking: Analysis of Classroom Interactions with 9 -Year- Old Students that Support the Transition from the Known to the Novel , 2007 .

[14]  Christopher B. Wolfe,et al.  Which Preschool Mathematics Competencies Are Most Predictive of Fifth Grade Achievement? , 2016, Early childhood research quarterly.

[15]  M. Mitchelmore,et al.  Awareness of pattern and structure in early mathematical development , 2009 .

[16]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Spontaneous Focusing on Quantitative Relations: Towards a Characterization , 2017 .

[17]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Gender equality in 4- to 5-year-old preschoolers' early numerical competencies. , 2018, Developmental science.

[18]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Four-year olds’ understanding of repeating and growing patterns and its association with early numerical ability , 2019, Early Childhood Research Quarterly.

[19]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  The roles of patterning and spatial skills in early mathematics development , 2019, Early Childhood Research Quarterly.

[20]  J. Sarama,et al.  Other Content Domains , 2009, Learning and Teaching Early Math.

[21]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Working memory and individual differences in mathematics achievement: a longitudinal study from first grade to second grade. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Home numeracy experiences and children’s math performance in the early school years. , 2009 .

[23]  Michael S. C. Thomas,et al.  The developmental relations between spatial cognition and mathematics in primary school children. , 2018, Developmental science.

[24]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity and Mathematical Skills of Young Children. , 2005 .

[25]  E. Lehtinen,et al.  Spontaneous Focusing on Quantitative Relations in the Development of Children's Fraction Knowledge , 2014 .

[26]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Preschoolers' strategies for solving visual pattern tasks , 2015 .

[27]  P. Starkey,et al.  Enhancing young children’s mathematical knowledge through a pre-kindergarten mathematics intervention , 2004 .

[28]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Number sense growth in kindergarten: a longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. , 2006, Child development.

[29]  Emily R. Fyfe,et al.  Emerging Understanding of Patterning in 4-Year-Olds , 2013 .