Neglect-like behavior in healthy subjects

Evidence has been reported favoring the view of a dual mode of space representation for action and spatial cognition. While the dorsal system seems to be mainly involved in direct coding of space for action by means of several effector-specific representations, the ventral system appears to be responsible for more enduring and conscious representations underlying spatial cognition and awareness. In accordance with this view are recent studies documenting dissociations between exploratory and goal-directed movements in patients with brain damage. Patients with neglect exhibit a spatial bias of exploratory movements to the ipsilesional side, while goal-directed movements land precisely on target. The exploratory bias was found susceptible to asymmetric sensory stimulation such as caloric vestibular stimulation, inducing transient reduction of contralateral neglect. The present study compared exploratory and goal-directed hand movements in healthy subjects following cold caloric stimulation of the right vestibular organ. We observed a rightward shift of tactile exploration, while goal-directed pointing remained unaffected. Asymmetric vestibular stimulation in healthy subjects thus produced a neglect-like behavior with a similar dissociation between impaired exploratory and nonimpaired goal-directed hand movements. The stimulation provoked a further, very characteristic symptom of neglect patients: a deviation of spontaneous head orientation toward the right. The present observations strengthen substantially the assumption of different modes of space representation for action and spatial cognition in humans.

[1]  M. Perenin,et al.  Temporary remission of representational hemineglect through vestibular stimulation. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[2]  M. Perenin,et al.  Optic Ataxia and Unilateral Neglect: Clinical Evidence for Dissociable Spatial Functions in Posterior Parietal Cortex , 1997 .

[3]  M. Albert A simple test of visual neglect , 1973, Neurology.

[4]  H. Karnath,et al.  Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Hans-Otto Karnath,et al.  Goal-Directed Hand Movements Are Not Affected by the Biased Space Representation in Spatial Neglect , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behaviour of patients with spatial neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional anatomy of pointing and grasping in humans. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  M. Perenin Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects , 1997 .

[11]  Matthew Rizzo,et al.  Disordered sensorimotor transformations for reaching following posterior cortical lesions , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  A. Pouget,et al.  Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching , 2002, Cognition.

[13]  K. Heilman,et al.  Posterior neocortical systems subserving awareness and neglect. Neglect associated with superior temporal sulcus but not area 7 lesions. , 1994, Archives of neurology.

[14]  M. Perenin,et al.  Tactile exploration of peripersonal space in patients with neglect , 1998, Neuroreport.

[15]  M. Mesulam Functional Anatomy of Attention and Neglect , 2002 .

[16]  F. Chédru Space representation in unilateral spatial neglect. , 1976, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[17]  G. Antonucci,et al.  Effect of Optokinetic Stimulation in Patients with Visual Neglect , 1990, Cortex.

[18]  S. Tipper,et al.  Object-centered not scene-based visual neglect. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect , 2002 .

[20]  S. Bentin,et al.  Domain specificity versus expertise: factors influencing distinct processing of faces , 2002, Cognition.

[21]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[22]  O. Bock,et al.  Contribution of retinal versus extraretinal signals towards visual localization in goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  G. Ratcliff,et al.  Defective visual localization in focal brain wounds. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  Donald G. Stein,et al.  Effects of posterior parietal lesions on visually guided behavior in monkeys , 1978, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  A. Milner,et al.  A paradoxical improvement of misreaching in optic ataxia: new evidence for two separate neural systems for visual localization , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[26]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Space exploration in neglect. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  A. Milner,et al.  Spatial Bias in Visually-Guided Reaching and Bisection Following Right Cerebral Stroke , 1994, Cortex.

[28]  Masud Husain,et al.  Reaching movements may reveal the distorted topography of spatial representations after neglect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  A. Milner,et al.  Grasping the past delay can improve visuomotor performance , 2001, Current Biology.

[30]  H. Karnath,et al.  Spatial orientation and the representation of space with parietal lobe lesions. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  S. Ferber,et al.  Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.

[32]  Jürgen Konczak,et al.  Kinematics of goal-directed arm movements in neglect: Control of hand in space , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  A. Rubens Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect , 1985, Neurology.

[34]  R. LaMotte,et al.  Defects in accuracy of reaching after removal of posterior parietal cortex in monkeys , 1978, Brain Research.

[35]  C. Frith,et al.  Cerebral representations for egocentric space: functional-anatomical evidence from caloric vestibular stimulation and neck vibration , 2001, NeuroImage.

[36]  M. Perenin,et al.  Partial Remission of Hemiplegia and Somatoparaphrenia Through Vestibular Stimulation in a Case of Unilateral Neglect , 1992, Cortex.

[37]  M A Goodale,et al.  Kinematic analysis of limb movements in neuropsychological research: subtle deficits and recovery of function. , 1990, Canadian journal of psychology.

[38]  H. Karnath,et al.  Decrease of contralateral neglect by neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[40]  S. E. Black,et al.  Impaired visual search in patients with unilateral neglect: an oculographic analysis , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  H. Karnath,et al.  The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Visual pathways for object-oriented action and object recognition: functional anatomy with PET. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  P. Roland,et al.  Functional anatomy of reaching and visuomotor learning: a positron emission tomography study. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  A P Batista,et al.  Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. , 1999, Science.

[45]  E. Renzi,et al.  Space Exploration with and without the Aid of Vision in Hemisphere-Damaged Patients , 1989, Cortex.

[46]  The localization of an imaginary target under the influence of caloric vestibular stimulation in healthy adults , 2000, European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology.

[47]  D. F. Benson,et al.  Visual form agnosia , 2000 .

[48]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Visuomotor Transformations for Reaching to Memorized Targets: A PET Study , 1997, NeuroImage.

[49]  M. Fetter,et al.  Ocular exploration of space as a function of neck proprioceptive and vestibular input — observations in normal subjects and patients with spatial neglect after parietal lesions , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  T. R. Jordan,et al.  Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  Charles M. Butter,et al.  Impairments in orienting to visual stimuli in monkeys following unilateral lesions of the superior sulcal polysensory cortex , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  J. Crawford,et al.  Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  F. Chédru,et al.  Visual searching in normal and brain-damaged subjects (contribution to the study of unilateral inattention). , 1973, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.