Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals.

In the context of an imitation game, 12- and 18-month-old infants saw an adult do such things as make a toy mouse hop across a mat (with sound effects). In one condition (House), the adult ended by placing the mouse in a toy house, whereas in another condition (No House) there was no house present at the final location. Infants at both ages usually simply put the mouse in the house (ignoring the hopping motion and sound effects) in the House condition, presumably because they interpreted the adult's action in terms of this final goal and so ignored the behavioral means. In contrast, infants copied the adult's action (both the hopping motion and the sound effects) when no house was present, presumably because here infants saw the action itself as the adult's only goal. From very early, infants' social learning is flexible: infants focus on and copy either the end or the means of an adult action as required by the context.

[1]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1993 .

[2]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.

[3]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[4]  M. Tomasello CHAPTER 15 – Do Apes Ape? , 1996 .

[5]  C. Heyes,et al.  Social learning in animals : the roots of culture , 1996 .

[6]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .

[7]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[8]  R. Hobson,et al.  Imitation and identification in autism. , 1999, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[9]  G. Csibra,et al.  Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.

[10]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds , 1999 .

[11]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Twelve-Month-Old Infants Interpret Action in Context , 2000, Psychological science.

[12]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Children's coding of human action: cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. , 2000, Developmental science.

[13]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[14]  Susan C. Johnson,et al.  Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent , 2001 .

[15]  Stephen C. Want,et al.  How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of 'imitation' in children , 2002 .

[16]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. , 2002, Child development.

[17]  W. Prinz,et al.  The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .

[18]  A. Whiten,et al.  How do apes ape? , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[19]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.

[20]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Unwilling versus unable: infants' understanding of intentional action. , 2005, Developmental psychology.