Dorsal forebrain anomaly in Williams syndrome.

BACKGROUND Williams syndrome (WMS) is a rare neurogenetic condition with a behavioral phenotype that suggests a dorsal and/or ventral developmental dissociation, with deficits in dorsal but not the ventral hemispheric visual stream. A shortened extent of the dorsal central sulcus has been observed in autopsy specimens. OBJECTIVE To compare gross anatomical features between the dorsal and ventral portions of the cerebral hemispheres by examining the dorsal extent of the central sulcus in brain magnetic resonance images from a sample of subjects with WMS and age- and sex-matched control subjects. SUBJECTS Twenty-one subjects having clinically and genetically diagnosed WMS (mean +/- SD age, 28.9 +/- 7.9 years) were compared with 21 age- and sex-matched typically developing controls (mean +/- SD age, 28.8 +/- 7.9 years). DESIGN High-resolution structural magnetic resonance images were acquired. The extent of the central sulcus was qualitatively assessed via surface projections of the cerebral cortex. RESULTS The dorsal central sulcus is less likely to reach the interhemispheric fissure in subjects with WMS than in controls for both left (P< .001, chi(2) = 15.79) and right (P< .001, chi(2) = 12.95) hemispheres. No differences between the groups were found in the ventral extent of the central sulcus. CONCLUSIONS Anomalies in the dorsal region in patients with WMS are indicative of early neurodevelopmental problems affecting the development of the dorsal forebrain and are most likely related to the deficits in visuospatial ability and behavioral timing often observed in this condition.

[1]  R. Adolphs,et al.  II. Hypersociability in Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  U Bellugi,et al.  Corpus callosum morphology of Williams syndrome: relation to genetics and behavior. , 2001, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[3]  Terri Gullickson Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images. , 1995 .

[4]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Corpus callosum morphology of Williams syndrome: relation to genetics and behavior , 2001 .

[5]  U Bellugi,et al.  Analysis of cerebral shape in Williams syndrome. , 2001, Archives of neurology.

[6]  D. J. Cunningham,et al.  Contribution to the surface anatomy of the cerebral hemispheres . with A chapter upon cranio-cerebral topography , 1892 .

[7]  Marian Williams,et al.  Correlative Anatomy of the Nervous System , 1963 .

[8]  A. Damasio,et al.  The neural basis of language. , 1984, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity during timing behavior in the monkey , 1979, Brain Research.

[10]  M. Raichle,et al.  Linguistic processing. , 1997, International review of neurobiology.

[11]  M E Raichle,et al.  What words are telling us about the brain. , 1996, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[12]  Rumiko Matsuoka,et al.  VI. Genome Structure and Cognitive Map of Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  H. Damasio Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images , 1995 .

[14]  R. Karis,et al.  Localization of speech parameters by brain scan , 1976, Neurology.

[15]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Musical Abilities in Individuals with Williams Syndrome , 1998 .

[16]  C A Morris,et al.  Natural history of Williams syndrome: physical characteristics. , 1988, The Journal of pediatrics.

[17]  I. Biederman,et al.  High level object recognition without an anterior inferior temporal lobe , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  E. Crosby,et al.  Correlative Anatomy of the Nervous System , 1962 .

[19]  M. Critchley The parietal lobes , 1966 .

[20]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  I. The Neurocognitive Profile of Williams Syndrome: A Complex Pattern of Strengths and Weaknesses , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Sergent,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  Warren S. McCulloch,et al.  The isocortex of the chimpanzee. , 1950 .

[23]  K. Uğurbil,et al.  Neural correlates of visual form and visual spatial processing , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[24]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  V. Multi-Level Analysis of Cortical Neuroanatomy in Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Stephan Eliez,et al.  IV. Neuroanatomy of Williams Syndrome: A High-Resolution MRI Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Tarr,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area is Part of a Network that Processes Faces at the Individual Level , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  J. Lang Klinische Anatomie des Kopfes , 1981 .

[28]  G. Yeni-Komshian,et al.  Anatomical study of cerebral asymmetry in the temporal lobe of humans, chimpanzees, and rhesus monkeys. , 1976, Science.

[29]  O. Braddick,et al.  A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams' syndrome , 1997, Neuroreport.

[30]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  F. Gilles,et al.  Gyral development of the human brain. , 1977, Annals of Neurology.

[32]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  The evolution of the frontal lobes: a volumetric analysis based on three-dimensional reconstructions of magnetic resonance scans of human and ape brains. , 1997, Journal of human evolution.

[33]  E. de Divitiis,et al.  Luigi Rolando and his pioneering efforts to relate structure to function in the nervous system. , 1995, Journal of neurosurgery.

[34]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Modulation of cerebral blood-flow in the human auditory cortex during speech: role of motor-to-sensory discharges , 1996, NeuroImage.

[35]  小野 道夫,et al.  Atlas of the Cerebral Sulci , 1990 .

[36]  G. Bonin,et al.  The neocortex of Macaca mulatta , 1947 .