Altered activation in the action observation system during synchronization in high loneliness individuals

Abstract Lonely people tend to evaluate social exchanges negatively and to display difficulties in interactions. Interpersonal synchronization is crucial for achieving positive interactions, promoting affinity, closeness, and satisfaction. However, little is known about lonely individuals’ ability to synchronize and about their brain activity while synchronizing. Following the screening of 303 participants, we recruited 32 low and 32 high loneliness participants. They were scanned while engaged in movement synchronization, using a novel dyadic interaction paradigm. Results showed that high loneliness individuals exhibited a reduced ability to adapt their movement to their partner’s movement. Intriguingly, during movement adaptation periods, high loneliness individuals showed increased activation in the action observation (AO) system, specifically in the inferior frontal gyrus and the inferior parietal lobule. They did not show increased activation in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, which in the context of synchronization was suggested to be related to gap-monitoring. Based on these findings, we propose a model according to which lonely people may require stronger activation of their AO system for alignment, to compensate for some deficiency in their synchronization ability. Despite this hyperactivation, they still suffer from reduced synchronization capacity. Consequently, synchronization may be a relevant intervention area for the amelioration of loneliness.

[1]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Multi-band FMRI compromises detection of mesolimbic reward responses , 2021, NeuroImage.

[2]  Camarin E. Rolle,et al.  The Role of the Dorsal-Lateral Prefrontal Cortex in Reward Sensitivity During Approach-Avoidance Conflict. , 2021, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Opposing Association of Situational and Chronic Loneliness with Interpersonal Distance , 2021, Brain sciences.

[4]  J. Mishra,et al.  Neurobiology of loneliness: a systematic review , 2021, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[5]  D. Margulies,et al.  The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective , 2021, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[6]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Loneliness and the Social Brain: How Perceived Social Isolation Impairs Human Interactions , 2021, bioRxiv.

[7]  M. Barreto,et al.  Loneliness around the world: Age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness , 2021, Personality and individual differences.

[8]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Herding in human groups is related to high autistic traits , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[9]  A. Grob,et al.  Loneliness is associated with the subjective evaluation of but not daily dynamics in partner relationships , 2020, International Journal of Behavioral Development.

[10]  M. M. Chan,et al.  Differential mirror neuron system (MNS) activation during action observation with and without social-emotional components in autism: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2020, Molecular Autism.

[11]  B. Derntl,et al.  Do I feel or do I know? Neuroimaging meta-analyses on the multiple facets of empathy , 2020, Cortex.

[12]  René J. Huster,et al.  tDCS over the inferior frontal gyri and visual cortices did not improve response inhibition , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[13]  Annett Schirmer,et al.  Interactional synchrony: signals, mechanisms and benefits , 2020, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[14]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Approach-Avoidance Conflict in Major Depressive Disorder: Congruent Neural Findings in Humans and Nonhuman Primates , 2020, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  Jianfeng Feng,et al.  Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3 , 2020, NeuroImage.

[16]  P. Winkielman,et al.  The neuroethology of spontaneous mimicry and emotional contagion in human and non-human animals , 2020, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  M. Meyer,et al.  Self-Other Representation in the Social Brain Reflects Social Connection , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  P. Winkielman,et al.  Smile (but only deliberately) though your heart is aching: Loneliness is associated with impaired spontaneous smile mimicry , 2019, Social neuroscience.

[19]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Herding Brains: A Core Neural Mechanism for Social Alignment , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  J. Grafman,et al.  The lonely brain: evidence from studying patients with penetrating brain injury , 2018, Social neuroscience.

[21]  Elizabeth A. Necka,et al.  Loneliness and Social Internet Use: Pathways to Reconnection in a Digital World? , 2018, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[22]  M. Gendron,et al.  Bio-behavioral synchrony promotes the development of conceptualized emotions. , 2017, Current opinion in psychology.

[23]  Eva M. Klein,et al.  Loneliness in the general population: prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health , 2017, BMC Psychiatry.

[24]  C. Mevorach,et al.  Isolating response inhibition in the brain: Parietal versus frontal contribution , 2017, Cortex.

[25]  K. Cadenhead,et al.  Mirror Neurons in Psychiatric Disorders: from Neuroception to Bio-behavioral System Dysregulation , 2017, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[26]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Synchrony as an Adaptive Mechanism for Large-Scale Human Social Bonding , 2016 .

[27]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Loneliness and implicit attention to social threat: A high-performance electrical neuroimaging study , 2016, Cognitive neuroscience.

[28]  Florence Gaunet,et al.  Behavioural synchronization from an ethological perspective: Overview of its adaptive value , 2016, Adapt. Behav..

[29]  Oliver Josephs,et al.  Cohesion and Joint Speech: Right Hemisphere Contributions to Synchronized Vocal Production , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  B. Hanratty,et al.  Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for coronary heart disease and stroke: systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal observational studies , 2016, Heart.

[31]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: Insights from autism spectrum disorder , 2016, Cognition.

[32]  Qiang Wang,et al.  The Role of the Frontal and Parietal Cortex in Proactive and Reactive Inhibitory Control: A Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Study , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  M. Osaka,et al.  How Two Brains Make One Synchronized Mind in the Inferior Frontal Cortex: fNIRS-Based Hyperscanning During Cooperative Singing , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[34]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  White matter structures associated with loneliness in young adults , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[35]  Lior Noy,et al.  Being in the zone: physiological markers of togetherness in joint improvisation , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[36]  Tal-Chen Rabinowitch,et al.  Synchronous Rhythmic Interaction Enhances Children’s Perceived Similarity and Closeness towards Each Other , 2015, PloS one.

[37]  Tristen K. Inagaki,et al.  The role of the ventral striatum in inflammatory-induced approach toward support figures , 2015, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[38]  Stephanie Cacioppo,et al.  The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[39]  Laura K. Cirelli,et al.  Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. , 2014, Developmental science.

[40]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  You are in sync with me: Neural correlates of interpersonal synchrony with a partner , 2014, Neuroscience.

[41]  Peng Xu,et al.  White matter structure in loneliness: preliminary findings from diffusion tensor imaging , 2014, Neuroreport.

[42]  T. Hendler,et al.  The brain basis of social synchrony. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[43]  Stephanie Cacioppo,et al.  Evolutionary mechanisms for loneliness , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[44]  P. Janata,et al.  Being and feeling in sync with an adaptive virtual partner: brain mechanisms underlying dynamic cooperativity. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[46]  Freya Bailes,et al.  Synchronization can influence trust following virtual interaction. , 2013, Experimental psychology.

[47]  Robert A Schoevers,et al.  Feelings of loneliness, but not social isolation, predict dementia onset: results from the Amsterdam Study of the Elderly (AMSTEL) , 2012, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[48]  Chaozhe Zhu,et al.  Neural Synchronization during Face-to-Face Communication , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Marie T Banich,et al.  A brain network instantiating approach and avoidance motivation. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[50]  Ivan Toni,et al.  On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain” , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[51]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Decoding the invisible forces of social connections , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[52]  C. Victor,et al.  The Prevalence of Loneliness Among Adults: A Case Study of the United Kingdom , 2012, The Journal of psychology.

[53]  B. Fredrickson,et al.  Strangers in sync: Achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. , 2012, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[54]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Specifying the Neurobiological Basis of Human Attachment: Brain, Hormones, and Behavior in Synchronous and Intrusive Mothers , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[55]  Piercarlo Valdesolo,et al.  Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. , 2011, Emotion.

[56]  Louise C Hawkley,et al.  Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[57]  T. Vicsek,et al.  Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks , 2010, Nature.

[58]  Michael J. Hove,et al.  It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation , 2009 .

[59]  G. Dichter,et al.  Autism is characterized by dorsal anterior cingulate hyperactivation during social target detection. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[60]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Social Connection Through Joint Action and Interpersonal Coordination , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[61]  Jean Decety,et al.  In the Eye of the Beholder: Individual Differences in Perceived Social Isolation Predict Regional Brain Activation to Social Stimuli , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  Frank E. Pollick,et al.  Neural Substrates for Action Understanding at Different Description Levels in the Human Brain , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  Letitia Anne Peplau,et al.  Toward a Social Psychology of Loneliness , 2008 .

[64]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. , 2007, Human movement science.

[65]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[66]  Bar-Ilan Parent–Infant Synchrony Biological Foundations and Developmental Outcomes , 2007 .

[67]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Loneliness within a nomological net: An evolutionary perspective , 2006 .

[68]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[69]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[70]  Gary G Berntson,et al.  Loneliness in everyday life: cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[71]  John B. Nezlek,et al.  Psychological well-being and day-to-day social interaction among older adults , 2002 .

[72]  J C Mazziotta,et al.  Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[73]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[74]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Malesand Females, Scientists and Mathematicians , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[75]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[76]  D. Russell UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. , 1996, Journal of personality assessment.

[77]  Mark H. Davis Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. , 1983 .

[78]  D. Russell,et al.  The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. , 1980, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[79]  D. Russell,et al.  Developing a measure of loneliness. , 1978, Journal of personality assessment.

[80]  R. Weiss,et al.  Loneliness: The Experience of Emotional and Social Isolation. , 1975 .