Auditory detection and localization of approaching vehicles.

Pedestrians must use a variety of cues when making safe decisions, many of which require processing of auditory information. We examined detection and localization of approaching vehicles using auditory cues. 50 adults ages 18-49 were presented with actual sounds of vehicles approaching at 5, 12, 25, and 35 mph. Three indices were of interest: the distance at which vehicles were detected, participants' decision regarding the direction from which vehicles were approaching, and their determination of the vehicles' arrival at their location. Participants more easily detected vehicles moving at higher speeds and vehicles approaching from the right. Determination of the direction of approach reached 90% accuracy or better when vehicles were traveling at, or greater than, 12 mph, and were more approaching from the right. Determination of vehicle arrival deteriorated significantly as speeds increased. Implications of the use of auditory cues in pedestrian settings, and future directions, are discussed.

[1]  Michael Erb,et al.  Impact of task difficulty on lateralization of pitch and duration discrimination , 2005, Neuroreport.

[2]  Paul E. Ponchillia,et al.  Street crossing by sighted and blind pedestrians at a modern roundabout , 2005 .

[3]  Lucy Johnston,et al.  An investigation of road crossing in a virtual environment. , 2003, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[4]  M. Schönwiesner,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry for spectral and temporal processing in the human antero‐lateral auditory belt cortex , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  Benjamin K. Barton,et al.  Integrating selective attention into developmental pedestrian safety research. , 2006 .

[6]  D.A. Guth,et al.  Tests of Blind Pedestrians’ Use of Traffic Sounds for Street-crossing Alignment , 1989 .

[7]  K. Ampofo-Boateng,et al.  Child pedestrian accidents: a case for preventive medicine , 1990 .

[8]  K. Pfeffer,et al.  Children's auditory perception of movement of traffic sounds. , 1996, Child: care, health and development.

[9]  R. Norton,et al.  Effect of environmental factors on risk of injury of child pedestrians by motor vehicles: a case-control study , 1995, BMJ.

[10]  Dona Sauerburger,et al.  Detecting Approaching Vehicles at Streets with No Traffic Control , 2008, Journal of visual impairment & blindness.

[11]  Koorosh Naghshineh,et al.  The Impact of Hybrid Vehicles on Street Crossings. , 2006 .

[12]  T. Pitcairn,et al.  Errors in young children's decisions about traffic gaps: experiments with roadside simulations. , 1992, British journal of psychology.

[13]  Michael E R Nicholls,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries for visual and auditory temporal processing: an evoked potential study. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[14]  William M. Hartmann,et al.  Localization of sound in rooms. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  E R Hoffmann,et al.  Children's Estimates of Vehicle Approach Times , 1980, Human factors.

[16]  William M. Hartmann,et al.  How we localize sound , 1999 .

[17]  Paul E. Ponchillia,et al.  Blind and Sighted Pedestrians' Judgments of Gaps in Traffic at Roundabouts , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[18]  R. Hanau,et al.  A further report on physics teaching in Java , 1958 .

[19]  Frank W. Ohl,et al.  Global versus local processing of frequency-modulated tones in gerbils: An animal model of lateralized auditory cortex functions , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  P J Gruenewald,et al.  Demographic and environmental correlates of pedestrian injury collisions: a spatial analysis. , 2000, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[21]  David N. Lee,et al.  A roadside simulation of road crossing for children , 1984 .

[22]  Guohua Li,et al.  Injury prevention and control. , 2007, Emergency medicine clinics of North America.

[23]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involuntary orienting to sound improves visual perception , 2000, Nature.