SIHGT and SUNOD: The role of orthography and phonology in the perception of transposed letter anagrams
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. O. Robinson,et al. The role of bigram frequency in the perception of words and nonwords , 1975, Memory & cognition.
[2] M. Masson. Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. , 2003, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[3] Lynn E. Snyder,et al. College students with dyslexia: persistent linguistic deficits and foreign language learning. , 2000, Dyslexia.
[4] V. Standish. Skilled Reading with Impaired Phonology: A Case Study , 1996 .
[5] Shelley Miller-Shaul,et al. The characteristics of young and adult dyslexics readers on reading and reading related cognitive tasks as compared to normal readers. , 2005, Dyslexia.
[6] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Effects of Phonological and Orthographic Neighbourhood Density Interact in Visual Word Recognition , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[7] J. Grainger,et al. Phonology and Orthography in Visual Word Recognition: Evidence from Masked Non-Word Priming , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[8] K. Stanovich,et al. Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. , 1994 .
[9] R. H. Baayen,et al. The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .
[10] G. C. Orden,et al. Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[11] J. Raaijmakers,et al. How to deal with "The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy": Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. , 1999 .
[12] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and Performance VI , 1978 .
[13] Max Coltheart,et al. Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .
[14] H. Rubenstein,et al. Homographic entries in the internal lexicon , 1970 .
[15] G. C. Orden. A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading , 1987 .
[16] M. Bruck. Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits. , 1992 .
[17] Mark Yates,et al. Phonological neighbors speed visual word processing: evidence from multiple tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[18] C. Perfetti,et al. Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming , 1991 .
[19] D. Shankweiler,et al. Phonology and the Problems of Learning to Read and Write , 1985 .
[20] Padraic Monaghan,et al. Reading, sublexical units and scrambled words: Capturing the human data , 2004 .
[21] Maggie Bruck,et al. Component Spelling Skills of College Students with Childhood Diagnoses of Dyslexia , 1993 .
[22] S. Lupker,et al. Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .
[23] M T Turvey,et al. Phonological codes are early sources of constraint in visual semantic categorization , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] M Coltheart,et al. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.
[25] S. Lupker,et al. Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[26] M. Snowling,et al. The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. , 1980, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[27] U. Goswami,et al. Phonological Skills and Learning to Read , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[28] M. Bruck. Word Recognition and Component Phonological Processing Skills of Adults with Childhood Diagnosis of Dyslexia , 1993 .
[29] J. Ziegler,et al. Word superiority, pseudoword superiority, and learning to read: A comparison of dyslexic and normal readers , 2003, Brain and Language.
[30] K. Forster,et al. Criterion bias and search sequence bias in word recognition , 1981, Memory & cognition.
[31] D. Howard,et al. Developmental Phonological Dyslexia: Real Word Reading Can Be Completely Normal , 1996 .
[32] J. Grainger,et al. Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .
[33] G. Stone,et al. Strategic control of processing in word recognition. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] C. Whitney. How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[35] R. L. Majeres. Phonological and Orthographic Coding Skills in Adult Readers , 2005, The Journal of general psychology.
[36] M. Coltheart,et al. Varieties of developmental dyslexia , 1993, Cognition.
[37] K. Forster,et al. Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? , 1987 .
[38] D. Compton. The Relationships Among Phonological Processing, Orthographic Processing, and Lexical Development in Children with Reading Disabilities , 2002 .
[39] J. Rack,et al. Orthographic and phonetic coding in developmental dyslexia. , 1985, British journal of psychology.
[40] H. Rubenstein,et al. Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition , 1971 .
[41] J C Ziegler,et al. Pseudohomophone effects in lexical decision: still a challenge for current word recognition models. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] D W Massaro,et al. Visual, orthographic, phonological, and lexical influences in reading. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] R. Frost. Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: true issues and false trails. , 1998 .
[44] A. Pollatsek,et al. Evidence for the use of assembled phonology in accessing the meaning of printed words. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[45] K. Stanovich,et al. Patterns of word and nonword processing in skilled and less-skilled readers , 1999 .
[46] S. M. Chambers. Letter and order information in lexical access , 1979 .
[47] S. Andrews. Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .
[48] Chris Westbury,et al. The Probability of the Least Likely Non-Length-Controlled Bigram Affects Lexical Decision Reaction Times , 2002, Brain and Language.
[49] R. Campbell,et al. Phonological Dyslexia and Dysgraphia in a Highly Literate Subject: A Developmental Case with Associated Deficits of Phonemic Processing and Awareness , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[50] Uta Frith,et al. Phonological difficulties in high-functioning dyslexics , 1996 .
[51] Ron Borowsky,et al. Diagnostics of phonological lexical processing: Pseudohomophone naming advantages, disadvantages, and base-word frequency effects , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[52] M. Snowling,et al. Cognitive assessment of dyslexic students in higher education. , 2002, The British journal of educational psychology.
[53] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.
[54] L. Siegel,et al. Evidence for Superior Orthographic Skills in Dyslexics , 1995 .
[55] Colin J. Davis,et al. The self-organising lexical acquisition and recognition (SOLAR) model of visual word recognition. , 2001 .