Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study.

OBJECTIVE To investigate heritability and continuum versus categorical approaches to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), using a large-scale twin sample. METHOD A cohort of 1,938 families with twins and siblings aged 4 to 12 years, recruited from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Twin Registry, was assessed for ADHD using a DSM-III-R-based maternal rating scale. Probandwise concordance rates and correlations in monozygotic and dizygotic twins and siblings were calculated, and heritability was examined using the De Fries and Fulker regression technique. RESULTS There was a narrow (additive) heritability of 0.75 to 0.91 which was robust across familial relationships (twin, sibling, and twin-sibling) and across definitions of ADHD as part of a continuum or as a disorder with various symptom cutoffs. There was no evidence for nonadditive genetic variation or for shared family environmental effects. CONCLUSIONS These findings suggest that ADHD is best viewed as the extreme of a behavior that varies genetically throughout the entire population rather than as a disorder with discrete determinants. This has implications for the classification of ADHD and for the identification of genes for this behavior, as well as implications for diagnosis and treatment.

[1]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Behavior problems in 5-year-old monozygotic and dizygotic twins: Genetic and environmental influences, patterns of regulation, and internalization of control , 1996, Development and Psychopathology.

[2]  I Waldman,et al.  Twin sibling differences in parental reports of ADHD, speech, reading and behaviour problems. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[3]  J. Sundet,et al.  Genetic influence on parent-reported attention-related problems in a Norwegian general population twin sample. , 1996, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[4]  D. Hay,et al.  The Differential Diagnosis of ADHD , 1996 .

[5]  R. Plomin,et al.  Importance of nonshared environmental factors for childhood and adolescent psychopathology. , 1996, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[6]  J. Swanson,et al.  Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 1996, Molecular psychiatry.

[7]  K. Kendler Genetic epidemiology in psychiatry. Taking both genes and environment seriously. , 1995, Archives of general psychiatry.

[8]  P. McGuffin,et al.  Childhood hyperactivity scores are highly heritable and show sibling competition effects: Twin study evidence , 1995, Behavior genetics.

[9]  L. Thompson,et al.  A twin study of competence and problem behavior in childhood and early adolescence. , 1995, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[10]  D W Fulker,et al.  Quantitative trait locus for reading disability: correction. , 1995, Science.

[11]  A. Torres,et al.  Reading disability, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and the immune system. , 1995, Science.

[12]  N J Cox,et al.  Association of attention-deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene. , 1995, American journal of human genetics.

[13]  D W Fulker,et al.  Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on chromosome 6. , 1994, Science.

[14]  R. Todd Use of siblings, twins, and cousins in the study of child and adolescent psychopathology , 1994 .

[15]  M. Owen,et al.  The genetic basis of complex human behaviors. , 1994, Science.

[16]  J. Rodgers,et al.  A simple algebraic demonstration of the validity of DeFries-Fulker analysis in unselected samples with multiple kinship levels , 1994, Behavior genetics.

[17]  L. Hechtman Genetic and neurobiological aspects of attention deficit hyperactive disorder: a review. , 1994, Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN.

[18]  D. Boomsma,et al.  A study of problem behaviors in 10- to 15-year-old biologically related and unrelated international adoptees , 1994, Behavior genetics.

[19]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Nature, Nurture, and Psychology , 1993 .

[20]  P. Jensen,et al.  Scales, diagnoses, and child psychopathology: I. CBCL and DISC relationships. , 1993, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[21]  S. Faraone,et al.  Segregation analysis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder , 1992 .

[22]  J. Benjamin,et al.  Further evidence for family-genetic risk factors in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Patterns of comorbidity in probands and relatives psychiatrically and pediatrically referred samples. , 1992, Archives of general psychiatry.

[23]  L. Cardon,et al.  Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families , 1992 .

[24]  B. Pennington,et al.  Attention deficit disorder in reading-disabled twins: Evidence for a genetic etiology , 1992, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[25]  J. Stevenson Evidence for a genetic etiology in hyperactivity in children , 1992, Behavior genetics.

[26]  E. Taylor,et al.  The Epidemiology of Childhood Hyperactivity , 1991 .

[27]  T. Achenbach Manual for the child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile , 1991 .

[28]  M. Rutter Nature, nurture, and psychopathology: A new look at an old topic , 1991, Development and Psychopathology.

[29]  S. Faraone,et al.  Family-genetic and psychosocial risk factors in DSM-III attention deficit disorder. , 1990, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[30]  J. Stevenson,et al.  A twin study of hyperactivity--II. The aetiological role of genes, family relationships and perinatal adversity. , 1989, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[31]  D. Fulker,et al.  Multiple regression analysis of twin data: etiology of deviant scores versus individual differences. , 1988, Acta geneticae medicae et gemellologiae.

[32]  J. Biederman,et al.  Conduct and oppositional disorder in clinically referred children with attention deficit disorder: a controlled family study. , 1987, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[33]  D. Fulker,et al.  Multiple regression analysis of twin data , 1985, Behavior genetics.

[34]  M. Prior,et al.  PREDICTION OF READING DISABILITY TWIN BOYS , 1984, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[35]  D. Hay,et al.  The La Trobe Twin Study: a genetic approach to the structure and development of cognition in twin children. , 1983, Child development.

[36]  D. Cohen,et al.  Reliably separating identical from fraternal twins. , 1975, Archives of general psychiatry.

[37]  L. Willerman Activity level and hyperactivity in twins. , 1973, Child development.

[38]  D. Safer,et al.  A familial factor in minimal brain dysfunction , 1973, Behavior genetics.

[39]  M. Thiéry The epidemiology of multiple births in Europe , 1996 .

[40]  R. Ebstein,et al.  Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism associated with the human personality trait of Novelty Seeking , 1996, Nature Genetics.

[41]  D. Murphy,et al.  Population and familial association between the D4 dopamine receptor gene and measures of Novelty Seeking , 1996, Nature Genetics.

[42]  L. Hechtman Do they grow out of it? : long-term outcomes of childhood disorders , 1996 .

[43]  M. Thiéry,et al.  The epidemiology of multiple births , 1995 .

[44]  A. Pickles,et al.  Genes, personality, and psychopathology: A latent class analysis of liability to symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in twins. , 1993 .

[45]  A. Bailey,et al.  Genetic factors in child psychiatric disorders--II. Empirical findings. , 1990, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[46]  M. Spence,et al.  Resolving genetic models for the transmission of schizophrenia , 1985, Genetic epidemiology.

[47]  M. Rutter,et al.  Education, Health and Behaviour , 1970 .