Chapter 3 Space perception and intended action

Intended actions are movements that are planned and executed for the very purpose of producing a specific effect, namely, the movement's outcome. This chapter discusses the close relationship between space perception and the intentional control of action. Proceeding from the assumption that intended actions are controlled by the anticipation of their effects, it is argued that intentional control demands three preconditions: (1) The relationship between the movement and its outcomes has to be systematic. (2) The visual system must be able to experience and recognize the systematicity of this relationship. (3) Information about which effects actually can be produced at that moment must be available. The discussion of these preconditions leads to the conclusion that intentional control of action requires a representation of all possible changes of the proximal stimulation (i.e., the reafferences) that can be produced by active movements. Saccadic eye movements are used as an example, to show that the perceptual space meets that demand, because it can be conceived as the entirety of all sensory changes (reafferences) that can be produced by saccades at that moment. This description is based on a view of retinal change as a temporal rather than a spatial variation. It is argued that every momentary stimulus situation is represented perceptually by nothing other than by how it can be changed by active movements. The possible changes are what Gibson has termed affordances. The underlying invariance is the law by which the sensory state can be changed by intentions. Some consequences of this account are considered.

[1]  James G. Taylor The behavioral basis of perception , 1962 .

[2]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[3]  R. Remington Attention and saccadic eye movements. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  E L Thomas,et al.  Movements of the eye. , 1968, Scientific American.

[5]  L. Festinger,et al.  Efference and the conscious experience of perception , 1967 .

[6]  M. Richards,et al.  The Integration of a Child into a Social World , 1974 .

[7]  P. Wolff Wahrnehmungslernen durch Blickbewegungen , 1985 .

[8]  R. B. Freeman,et al.  Cognition and Motor Processes , 1983 .

[9]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[10]  J. Henderson Visual Attention and Eye Movement Control During Reading and Picture Viewing , 1992 .

[11]  W. Wundt,et al.  Grundzüge der physiologischen psyhcologie , 1893 .

[12]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Why don't we perceive our brain states? , 1992 .

[13]  P. Wolff Saccadic Eye Movements and Visual Stability: Preliminary Considerations Towards a Cognitive Approach , 1984 .

[14]  E. Mach Die analyse der empfindungen und das verhältniss des physischen zum psychischen , 1902 .

[15]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Chapter 2 On the perception of position , 1999 .

[16]  J. Findlay,et al.  The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[17]  R. Andersen,et al.  Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex , 1997, Nature.

[18]  G. Roth,et al.  Kopf-Arbeit. Gehirnfunktionen und kognitive Leistungen , 1996 .

[19]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .

[20]  H. Heuer,et al.  Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .

[21]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[22]  J. Senders,et al.  Eye Movements and Psychological Processes , 1976 .

[23]  L. Arend Spatial differential and integral operations in human vision: implications of stabilized retinal image fading. , 1973, Psychological review.

[24]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[25]  K. Rayner Eye Movements and Visual Cognition , 1992 .

[26]  Robert M. Steinman,et al.  The extraretinal signal for the visual perception of direction , 1972 .

[27]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Organization of monkey superior colliculus: enhanced visual response of superficial layer cells. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  G. Brindley,et al.  The absence of position sense in the human eye , 1960, The Journal of physiology.

[30]  Odmar Neumann,et al.  Perspektiven der Kognitionspsychologie , 1985 .

[31]  Ernst Mach,et al.  The analysis of sensations and the relation of the physical to the psychical , 1914, The Mathematical Gazette.

[32]  T. Bachmann,et al.  Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (Advances in Psychology, Vol. 129) , 1999 .

[33]  W. B. Templeton,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  E. Matin Saccadic suppression: a review and an analysis. , 1974, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  R. Klein,et al.  Does Oculomotor Readiness Mediate Cognitive Control of Visual-Attention - Revisited , 1994 .

[36]  F. Brentano Psychologie vom empirischen Standpunkte , 1874 .

[37]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[38]  W. Wolf,et al.  Corrective saccades: Effect of shifting the saccade goal , 1982, Vision Research.

[39]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[40]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung , 1998 .

[41]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. , 1970, Psychological review.

[42]  I. Howard,et al.  Human Spatial Orientation , 1966 .

[43]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Relative mislocalization of briefly presented stimuli in the retinal periphery , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  R S Slotnick,et al.  Adaptation to curvature distortion. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[45]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[46]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[47]  W. Becker Do correction saccades depend exclusively on retinal feedback? A note on the possible role of non-retinal feedback , 1976, Vision Research.

[48]  R Kawashima,et al.  Topographic representation in human intraparietal sulcus of reaching and saccade , 1996, Neuroreport.

[49]  A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al.  Selective Attention in Vision , 1991 .

[50]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.