Mu rhythm desynchronization is specific to action execution and observation: Evidence from time-frequency and connectivity analysis

&NA; Mu desynchronization is the attenuation of EEG power in the alpha frequency range recorded over central scalp locations thought to reflect motor cortex activation. Mu desynchronization during observation of an action is believed to reflect mirroring system activation in humans. However, this notion has recently been questioned because, among other reasons, the potential contamination of mu rhythm and occipital alpha activity induced by attention processes following presentation of visual stimuli in observation conditions. This study examined the validity of mu desynchronization as a measure of mirroring system activation in infants and further investigated the pattern of functional connectivity between the central and occipital regions during execution and observation of movement. EEG was recorded while 46 9‐month‐old infants executed grasping actions and observed an experimenter grasping. Current source density (CSD) was applied to EEG data and, time‐frequency and connectivity analyses were performed in CSD transformed data. Mu desynchronization was evident over central regions during both execution and observation of movements. Independent alpha desynchronization over occipital region was also present in both conditions. The connectivity analyses revealed that central‐occipital areas were functionally more connected compared to other areas of the brain during observation of movements. Collectively, the results demonstrate the validity of mu desynchronization as an index of infant mirroring system activity and support the proposal of a functional connection between distinct mirroring and attention processes during observation of action. HighlightsObservation of a movement evoked mirroring activity in infants.Mirroring activity began before observed movement suggesting anticipation the movement.Motor and occipital areas were functionally coupled during movement observation.Mu desynchronization can be used as a valid index of mirroring activity in infant.

[1]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  N. Fox,et al.  Action mechanisms for social cognition: behavioral and neural correlates of developing Theory of Mind. , 2017, Developmental science.

[3]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Is the mirror neuron system involved in imitation? A short review and meta-analysis , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  Joseph P. McCleery,et al.  EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Neural correlates of action observation and execution in 14-month-old infants: an event-related EEG desynchronization study. , 2011, Developmental science.

[6]  J. Pineda The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing” , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[7]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants , 2009, Biology Letters.

[8]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Foot and hand area mu rhythms. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[9]  Manuel Rodriguez,et al.  Mu-rhythm changes during the planning of motor and motor imagery actions , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Kathryn H. Yoo,et al.  Desynchronization in EEG during perception of means-end actions and relations with infants' grasping skill. , 2016, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[11]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Functional dissociation of lower and upper frequency mu rhythms in relation to voluntary limb movement , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[13]  L. Craighero,et al.  Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others’ action , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[14]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Bishop,et al.  Mu suppression – A good measure of the human mirror neuron system? , 2016, Cortex.

[16]  Jürgen Kayser,et al.  On the benefits of using surface Laplacian (current source density) methodology in electrophysiology. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[17]  H. Théoret,et al.  EEG evidence for the presence of an action observation–execution matching system in children , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  Gregory Hickok,et al.  Eight Problems for the Mirror Neuron Theory of Action Understanding in Monkeys and Humans , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  David J. Heeger,et al.  A mirror up to nature , 2008 .

[20]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  An EEG study on the somatotopic organisation of sensorimotor cortex activation during action execution and observation in infancy , 2015, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Infants predict other people's action goals , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  S. Bentin,et al.  Dynamics of the EEG power in the frequency and spatial domains during observation and execution of manual movements , 2013, Brain Research.

[27]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[28]  G. Csibra,et al.  'Obsessed with goals': functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[29]  R. B. Reilly,et al.  FASTER: Fully Automated Statistical Thresholding for EEG artifact Rejection , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[30]  Kerstin Rosander,et al.  Using mu rhythm desynchronization to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. , 2011, Developmental science.

[31]  A. Woodward,et al.  Motor System Activation Predicts Goal Imitation in 7-Month-Old Infants , 2016, Psychological science.

[32]  Vilayanur S. Ramachandran,et al.  EEG evidence for mirror neuron activity during the observation of human and robot actions: Toward an analysis of the human qualities of interactive robots , 2007, Neurocomputing.

[33]  M. Iacoboni Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[34]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  Kathryn H. Yoo,et al.  The mu-rhythm can mirror: Insights from experimental design, and looking past the controversy , 2017, Cortex.

[37]  V. Gallese The ‘‘shared manifold’’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy , 2001 .

[38]  Mercedes Atienza,et al.  State-modulation of cortico-cortical connections underlying normal EEG alpha variants , 2000, Physiology & Behavior.

[39]  Lisa M Oakes,et al.  It clicks when it is rolled and it squeaks when it is squeezed: what 10-month-old infants learn about object function. , 2006, Child development.

[40]  Kathryn H. Yoo,et al.  The Infant EEG Mu Rhythm: Methodological Considerations and Best Practices. , 2014, Developmental review : DR.

[41]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object , 2003 .

[42]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror system and its role in social cognition , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[43]  N. Fox,et al.  Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[44]  C. Tenke,et al.  Generator localization by current source density (CSD): Implications of volume conduction and field closure at intracranial and scalp resolutions , 2012, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[45]  M. Iacoboni Neurobiology of imitation , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[47]  P. Nunez,et al.  Comparison of the effect of volume conduction on EEG coherence with the effect of field spread on MEG coherence , 2007, Statistics in medicine.

[48]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.

[49]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[50]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Language Acquisition: Do as You Hear , 2002, Current Biology.

[51]  A. Mognon,et al.  ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[52]  A. Woodward,et al.  Relations between infants' emerging reach-grasp competence and event-related desynchronization in EEG. , 2016, Developmental science.

[53]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[55]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Neural mirroring mechanisms and imitation in human infants , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[56]  Luca Turella,et al.  Mirror neurons in humans: Consisting or confounding evidence? , 2009, Brain and Language.

[57]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Changes in rolandic mu rhythm during observation of a precision grip. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[58]  Bin He,et al.  Electrophysiological Imaging of Brain Activity and Connectivity—Challenges and Opportunities , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[59]  R. Lesser,et al.  Functional mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with electrocorticographic spectral analysis. I. Alpha and beta event-related desynchronization. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[60]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action , 2002 .

[62]  Jürgen Kayser,et al.  Issues and considerations for using the scalp surface Laplacian in EEG/ERP research: A tutorial review. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[63]  N. Fox,et al.  Spectral and source structural development of mu and alpha rhythms from infancy through adulthood , 2016, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[64]  R. Hari,et al.  Functional Segregation of Movement-Related Rhythmic Activity in the Human Brain , 1995, NeuroImage.

[65]  Gert Pfurtscheller,et al.  Desynchronization and recovery of β rhythms during brisk and slow self-paced finger movements in man , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[66]  S. Makeig,et al.  Mining event-related brain dynamics , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[67]  Joydeep Bhattacharya,et al.  Phase synchrony analysis of EEG during music perception reveals changes in functional connectivity due to musical expertise , 2005, Signal Process..

[68]  P. Grieve,et al.  Variation in infant EEG power across social and nonsocial contexts. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[69]  Kathryn H. Yoo,et al.  Assessing human mirror activity with EEG mu rhythm: A meta-analysis. , 2016, Psychological bulletin.

[70]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Event-related synchronization of mu rhythm in the EEG over the cortical hand area in man , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[71]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Effects of time lag and frequency matching on phase-based connectivity , 2015, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[72]  Heidi A. Baumgartner,et al.  Infants' Developing Sensitivity to Object Function: Attention to Features and Feature Correlations , 2011 .

[73]  Jürgen Kayser,et al.  Principal components analysis of Laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying ERP generator patterns: I. Evaluation with auditory oddball tasks , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[74]  P. Ferrari,et al.  Early maternal mirroring predicts infant motor system activation during facial expression observation , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[75]  Sabine Hunnius,et al.  The infant motor system predicts actions based on visual statistical learning , 2017, NeuroImage.

[76]  D. Bishop,et al.  Reply to Bowman et al.: Building the foundations for moving mu suppression research forward , 2017, Cortex.

[77]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Evaluation of event-related desynchronization (ERD) preceding and following voluntary self-paced movement. , 1979, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[78]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Brain regions with mirror properties: A meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[79]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Asymmetric fMRI adaptation reveals no evidence for mirror neurons in humans , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[80]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Actor's and observer's primary motor cortices stabilize similarly after seen or heard motor actions , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.