How Task Interactivity Shapes Action Observation.

Action observation triggers imitation, a powerful mechanism permitting interpersonal coordination. Coordination, however, also occurs when the partners' actions are nonimitative and physically incongruent. One influential theory postulates that this is achieved via top-down modulation of imitation exerted by prefrontal regions. Here, we rather argue that coordination depends on sharing a goal with the interacting partner: this shapes action observation, overriding involuntary imitation, through the predictive activity of the left ventral premotor cortex (lvPMc). During functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), participants played music in turn with a virtual partner in interactive and noninteractive conditions requiring 50% of imitative/nonimitative responses. In a full-factorial design, both perceptual features and low-level motor requirements were kept constant throughout the experiment. Behaviorally, the interactive context minimized visuomotor interference due to the involuntary imitation of physically incongruent movements. This was paralleled by modulation of neural activity in the lvPMc, which was specifically recruited during the interactive task independently of the imitative/nonimitative nature of the social exchange. This lvPMc activity reflected the predictive decoding of the partner's actions, as revealed by multivariate pattern analysis. This demonstrates that, during interactions, we process our partners' behavior to prospectively infer their contribution to the shared goal achievement, generating motor predictions for cooperation beyond low-level imitation.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[2]  Janeen D. Loehr,et al.  The sound of you and me: Novices represent shared goals in joint action , 2016, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Lauren V. Hadley,et al.  Causal Role of Motor Simulation in Turn-Taking Behavior , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[5]  C. Frith,et al.  Social cognition in the we-mode , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Acting together in and beyond the mirror neuron system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.

[8]  E. Paulesu,et al.  A Breakdown of Imagined Visuomotor Transformations and Its Neural Correlates in Young Elderly Subjects , 2018, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  D. Kourtis,et al.  Evidence for we-representations during joint action planning , 2019, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Giacomo Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror mechanism: a basic principle of brain function , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Brass,et al.  Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer,et al.  Neural correlates of observing joint actions with shared intentions , 2015, Cortex.

[13]  Marco Iacoboni,et al.  Multimodal action representation in human left ventral premotor cortex , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[14]  E. D. Paolo,et al.  Can social interaction constitute social cognition? , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  D. Kourtis,et al.  Predictive representation of other people's actions in joint action planning: An EEG study , 2013, Social neuroscience.

[16]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Boosting and Decreasing Action Prediction Abilities Through Excitatory and Inhibitory tDCS of Inferior Frontal Cortex , 2018, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  M. Candidi,et al.  Come together: human–avatar on-line interactions boost joint-action performance in apraxic patients , 2017, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[18]  Giacomo Novembre,et al.  Motor simulation and the coordination of self and other in real-time joint action. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[19]  Joint response–effect compatibility , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[21]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Shared Representations: Sensorimotor Foundations of Social Life , 2016 .

[22]  Helen J. Wall,et al.  Action-effect sharing induces task-set sharing in joint task switching , 2017, Cognition.

[23]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[24]  C. Liston,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[25]  Natacha S. Santos,et al.  Perceiving nonverbal behavior: Neural correlates of processing movement fluency and contingency in dyadic interactions , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[26]  S. Schütz-Bosbach,et al.  Mirror and (absence of) counter-mirror responses to action sounds measured with TMS , 2017, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[27]  Eraldo Paulesu,et al.  Evidence for a dyadic motor plan in joint action , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[28]  M. Candidi,et al.  Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior , 2017, Molecular Autism.

[29]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  G. R. Müller-Putz,et al.  Cooperation in mind: Motor imagery of joint and single actions is represented in different brain areas , 2016, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  Stephen A. Butterfill,et al.  Joint action and development , 2012 .

[32]  M. Candidi,et al.  From muscles synergies and individual goals to interpersonal synergies and shared goals: mirror neurons and interpersonal action hierarchies: comment on "Grasping synergies: a motor-control approach to the mirror neuron mechanism" by D'Ausilio et al. , 2015, Physics of life reviews.

[33]  C. Fiebach,et al.  Dynamic Anticipatory Processing of Hierarchical Sequential Events: a Common Role for Broca's Area and Ventral Premotor Cortex Across Domains? , 2006, Cortex.

[34]  D Yves von Cramon,et al.  Sequences of Abstract Nonbiological Stimuli Share Ventral Premotor Cortex with Action Observation and Imagery , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Janeen D. Loehr,et al.  Monitoring Individual and Joint Action Outcomes in Duet Music Performance , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  Yi Chen,et al.  Statistical inference and multiple testing correction in classification-based multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA): Random permutations and cluster size control , 2011, NeuroImage.

[37]  R. Cunnington,et al.  More than an imitation game: Top-down modulation of the human mirror system , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[38]  Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al.  Comparison of multivariate classifiers and response normalizations for pattern-information fMRI , 2010, NeuroImage.

[39]  Robert L. Whitwell,et al.  Predictive joint-action model: A hierarchical predictive approach to human cooperation , 2018, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  E. Saltzman,et al.  Action Representation of Sound: Audiomotor Recognition Network While Listening to Newly Acquired Actions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in the Control of Imitation , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Matteo Candidi,et al.  Causative role of left aIPS in coding shared goals during human–avatar complementary joint actions , 2015, Nature Communications.

[43]  B. Hommel Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding) , 2009, Psychological research.

[44]  Simone Schütz-Bosbach,et al.  Sensory attenuation of self‐produced signals does not rely on self‐specific motor predictions , 2018, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  M. Husain,et al.  Control over Conflict during Movement Preparation: Role of Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2008, Neuron.

[46]  Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al.  Anatomical substrates of cooperative joint-action in a continuous motor task: Virtual lifting and balancing , 2008, NeuroImage.

[47]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[48]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[49]  Cordula Vesper,et al.  Are you ready to jump? Predictive mechanisms in interpersonal coordination. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  I Know What You Are Doing A Neurophysiological Study , 2001, Neuron.

[51]  James M. Kilner,et al.  More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  Musical ensemble performance : representing self, other, and joint action outcomes , 2016 .

[53]  A. Battaglia-Mayer,et al.  Two Brains in Action: Joint-Action Coding in the Primate Frontal Cortex , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Katy A. Cross,et al.  To imitate or not: Avoiding imitation involves preparatory inhibition of motor resonance , 2014, NeuroImage.

[55]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  M. Candidi,et al.  Inhibition of left anterior intraparietal sulcus shows that mutual adjustment marks dyadic joint-actions in humans , 2018, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[57]  Antonino Casile,et al.  When Sounds Become Actions: Higher-order Representation of Newly Learned Action Sounds in the Human Motor System , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Transitory Inhibition of the Left Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus Impairs Joint Actions: A Continuous Theta-Burst Stimulation Study , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.