Eye movement planning on Single-Sensor-Single-Indicator displays is vulnerable to user anxiety and cognitive load

In this study, we demonstrate the effects of anxiety and cognitive load on eye movement planning in an instrument flight task adhering to a single-sensor-single-indicator data visualisation design philosophy. The task was performed in neutral and anxiety conditions, while a low or high cognitive load, auditory n-back task was also performed. Cognitive load led to a reduction in the number of transitions between instruments, and impaired task performance. Changes in self-reported anxiety between the neutral and anxiety conditions positively correlated with changes in the randomness of eye movements between instruments, but only when cognitive load was high. Taken together, the results suggest that both cognitive load and anxiety impact gaze behavior, and that these effects should be explored when designing data visualization displays

[1]  Miwa Hayashi,et al.  Characterizing Scan Patterns in a Spacecraft Cockpit Simulator: Expert Vs. Novice Performance , 2005 .

[2]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  State anxiety and visual attention: The role of the quiet eye period in aiming to a far target , 2008, Journal of sports sciences.

[3]  Elizabeth J Edwards,et al.  Effects of trait anxiety and situational stress on attentional shifting are buffered by working memory capacity , 2015, Anxiety, stress, and coping.

[4]  I. Sarason,et al.  Stress, anxiety, and cognitive interference: reactions to tests. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  David O'Hare,et al.  Can a glass cockpit display help (or hinder) performance of novices in simulated flight training? , 2015, Applied ergonomics.

[6]  Kim J. Vicente,et al.  Ecological interface design: theoretical foundations , 1992, IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern..

[7]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in within-subject designs. , 2001, Psychological methods.

[8]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. , 2007, Emotion.

[9]  C. Spielberger Theory and research on anxiety , 1966 .

[10]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Working memory capacity, controlled attention and aiming performance under pressure , 2016, Psychological research.

[12]  Cody Ding,et al.  Impact of Working Memory Load on Cognitive Control in Trait Anxiety: An ERP Study , 2014, PloS one.

[13]  Jonathan Allsop,et al.  Flying under pressure: Effects of anxiety on attention and gaze behavior in aviation , 2014 .

[14]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  The influence of anxiety on visual attentional control in basketball free throw shooting. , 2009, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[15]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  Ying Wang,et al.  A Field Study on the Impact of Variations in Short-Term Memory Demands on Drivers’ Visual Attention and Driving Performance Across Three Age Groups , 2012, Hum. Factors.

[17]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  The role of effort in influencing the effect of anxiety on performance: testing the conflicting predictions of processing efficiency theory and the conscious processing hypothesis. , 2007, British journal of psychology.

[18]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Individual reactions to stress predict performance during a critical aviation incident , 2015, Anxiety, stress, and coping.

[19]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  S R Ellis,et al.  Statistical Dependency in Visual Scanning , 1986, Human factors.

[21]  J. Causer,et al.  Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. , 2011, Emotion.

[22]  R. Gray,et al.  Changes in putting kinematics associated with choking and excelling under pressure. , 2013 .

[23]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Quiet eye training expedites motor learning and aids performance under heightened anxiety: the roles of response programming and external attention. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[24]  M. A. Recarte,et al.  Mental workload while driving: effects on visual search, discrimination, and decision making. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[25]  L. P. Goodstein,et al.  Discriminative Display Support for Process Operators , 1981 .

[26]  C. Grillon,et al.  Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: from impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[27]  Joseph H. Goldberg,et al.  Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols , 2000, ETRA.

[28]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  A Fixed-Based Flight Simulator Study: The Interdependence of Flight Control Performance and Gaze Efficiency , 2013, HCI.

[29]  David Woods,et al.  An Evaluation of Nuclear Power Plant Safety Parameter Display Systems , 1981 .

[30]  A. Williams,et al.  The Effects of Anxiety on Visual Search, Movement Kinematics, and Performance in Table Tennis: A Test of Eysenck and Calvo's Processing Efficiency Theory , 2001 .

[31]  W. Kirchner Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  N. Berggren,et al.  Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: Why you see them and why you don’t , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[33]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Working Memory Capacity, Visual Attention and Hazard Perception in Driving , 2016 .

[34]  Nick Berggren,et al.  Affective attention under cognitive load: reduced emotional biases but emergent anxiety-related costs to inhibitory control , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[35]  David McIntyre,et al.  Effects of competitive pressure on expert performance: underlying psychological, physiological, and kinematic mechanisms. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[36]  D. Rubin,et al.  Comparing Correlated but Nonoverlapping Correlations , 1996 .

[37]  Roger W. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Visual Misperception in Aviation: Glide Path Performance in a Black Hole Environment , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[38]  Kim J. Vicente,et al.  Designing Functional Visualizations for Aircraft Systems Status Displays , 1999 .

[39]  W. Russell,et al.  Measuring Anxiety in Athletics: The Revised Competitive State Anxiety Inventory–2 , 2003 .

[40]  R. Oudejans,et al.  Effects of anxiety, a cognitive secondary task, and expertise on gaze behavior and performance in a far aiming task , 2012 .

[41]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Anxiety, attentional control, and performance impairment in penalty kicks. , 2009, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[42]  John M. Flach,et al.  Beyond Ecological Interface Design: Lessons From Concerns and Misconceptions , 2015, IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems.

[43]  S. Gronlund,et al.  Individuals lower in working memory capacity are particularly vulnerable to anxiety's disruptive effect on performance , 2009, Anxiety, stress, and coping.

[44]  R L Harris,et al.  Visual scanning behavior and mental workload in aircraft pilots. , 1982, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[45]  J A Effken,et al.  Making the constraints visible: testing the ecological approach to interface design. , 1997, Ergonomics.

[46]  Luis de Rivera [Stress and anxiety]. , 1995, Actas luso-espanolas de neurologia, psiquiatria y ciencias afines.

[47]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Effects of anxiety and cognitive load on instrument scanning behavior in a flight simulation , 2016, 2016 IEEE Second Workshop on Eye Tracking and Visualization (ETVIS).