Inter-Individual Differences in Neurobehavioural Impairment following Sleep Restriction Are Associated with Circadian Rhythm Phase

Although sleep restriction is associated with decrements in daytime alertness and neurobehavioural performance, there are considerable inter-individual differences in the degree of impairment. This study examined the effects of short-term sleep restriction on neurobehavioural performance and sleepiness, and the associations between individual differences in impairments and circadian rhythm phase. Healthy adults (n = 43; 22 M) aged 22.5 ± 3.1 (mean ± SD) years maintained a regular 8:16 h sleep:wake routine for at least three weeks prior to laboratory admission. Sleep opportunity was restricted to 5 hours time-in-bed at home the night before admission and 3 hours time-in-bed in the laboratory, aligned by wake time. Hourly saliva samples were collected from 5.5 h before until 5 h after the pre-laboratory scheduled bedtime to assess dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) as a marker of circadian phase. Participants completed a 10-min auditory Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT), the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) and had slow eye movements (SEM) measured by electrooculography two hours after waking. We observed substantial inter-individual variability in neurobehavioural performance, particularly in the number of PVT lapses. Increased PVT lapses (r = -0.468, p < 0.01), greater sleepiness (r = 0.510, p < 0.0001), and more slow eye movements (r = 0.375, p = 0.022) were significantly associated with later DLMO, consistent with participants waking at an earlier circadian phase. When the difference between DLMO and sleep onset was less than 2 hours, individuals were significantly more likely to have at least three attentional lapses the following morning. This study demonstrates that the phase of an individual’s circadian system is an important variable in predicting the degree of neurobehavioural performance impairment in the hours after waking following sleep restriction, and confirms that other factors influencing performance decrements require further investigation.

[1]  M. Carskadon,et al.  Regulation of Adolescent Sleep: Implications for Behavior , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  F. Scheer,et al.  An Endogenous Circadian Rhythm in Sleep Inertia Results in Greatest Cognitive Impairment upon Awakening during the Biological Night , 2008, Journal of biological rhythms.

[3]  C. Czeisler,et al.  Exposure to Room Light before Bedtime Suppresses Melatonin Onset and Shortens Melatonin Duration in Humans Setting: Participants Lived in a General Clinical Research Center for at Least Five Consecutive Days , 2022 .

[4]  D. Dinges,et al.  Neurocognitive Consequences of Sleep Deprivation , 2005, Seminars in neurology.

[5]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Sleepiness and Performance in Response to Repeated Sleep Restriction and Subsequent Recovery during Semi‐Laboratory Conditions , 2008, Chronobiology international.

[6]  Hans P. A. Van Dongen,et al.  Morning-type and evening-type individuals differ in the phase position of their endogenous circadian oscillator , 1996, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  D. Dijk,et al.  Alertness, mood and performance rhythm disturbances associated with circadian sleep disorders in the blind , 2008, Journal of sleep research.

[8]  Josephine Arendt,et al.  A length polymorphism in the circadian clock gene Per3 is linked to delayed sleep phase syndrome and extreme diurnal preference. , 2003, Sleep.

[9]  T. Monk Sleep, sleepiness, and performance , 1991 .

[10]  G. Kerkhof The 24-Hour Variation of Mood Differs between Morning- and Evening-Type Individuals , 1998, Perceptual and motor skills.

[11]  John A Groeger,et al.  Early morning executive functioning during sleep deprivation is compromised by a PERIOD3 polymorphism. , 2008, Sleep.

[12]  W. Revelle,et al.  Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: with an emphasis on morningness–eveningness , 2000, Journal of sleep research.

[13]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Intrinsic near-24-h pacemaker period determines limits of circadian entrainment to a weak synchronizer in humans , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  D. Dinges,et al.  Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. , 2004, Sleep.

[15]  Daniel Aeschbach,et al.  A longer biological night in long sleepers than in short sleepers. , 2003, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[16]  J. Horne,et al.  A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. , 1976, International journal of chronobiology.

[17]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Functional Genomics of Sleep and Circadian Rhythm Invited Review : Integration of human sleep-wake regulation and circadian rhythmicity , 2001 .

[18]  A. Pack,et al.  Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. , 1997, Sleep.

[19]  W. F. Waters,et al.  Short-term total sleep deprivations does not selectively impair higher cortical functioning. , 1999, Sleep.

[20]  C. Eastman,et al.  SLEEP LOGS OF YOUNG ADULTS WITH SELF-SELECTED SLEEP TIMES PREDICT THE DIM LIGHT MELATONIN ONSET , 2002, Chronobiology international.

[21]  C. Schmidt,et al.  A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[22]  J. Carrier,et al.  Circadian and homeostatic sleep regulation in morningness–eveningness , 2006, Journal of sleep research.

[23]  D. Dinges,et al.  Performing while sleepy: Effects of experimentally-induced sleepiness. , 1991 .

[24]  D. Dawson,et al.  The Relationship Between the Dim Light Melatonin Onset and Sleep on a Regular Schedule in Young Healthy Adults , 2003, Behavioral sleep medicine.

[25]  J. Horne,et al.  Gender- and age-related differences in sleep determined by home-recorded sleep logs and actimetry from 400 adults. , 1995, Sleep.

[26]  Elizabeth B Klerman,et al.  Peak of circadian melatonin rhythm occurs later within the sleep of older subjects. , 2002, American journal of physiology. Endocrinology and metabolism.

[27]  E. Mignot,et al.  PER3 Polymorphism Predicts Cumulative Sleep Homeostatic but Not Neurobehavioral Changes to Chronic Partial Sleep Deprivation , 2009, PloS one.

[28]  Pietro Badia,et al.  Inter‐ and intra‐individual variability in performance near the circadian nadir during sleep deprivation , 2004, Journal of sleep research.

[29]  Claude Gronfier,et al.  Intrinsic Period and Light Intensity Determine the Phase Relationship between Melatonin and Sleep in Humans , 2005, Journal of biological rhythms.

[30]  D. Dijk,et al.  PER3 Polymorphism Predicts Sleep Structure and Waking Performance , 2007, Current Biology.

[31]  S. Doran,et al.  Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. , 2001, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[32]  C. Czeisler,et al.  Healthy Older Adults Better Tolerate Sleep Deprivation Than Young Adults , 2009, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[33]  D. Dinges,et al.  Sleep debt: Theoretical and empirical issues , 2003 .

[34]  A. Lewy,et al.  Melatonin rhythms in night shift workers. , 1992, Sleep.

[35]  D. Dinges,et al.  The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. , 2003, Sleep.

[36]  P. Lavie,et al.  Ultrashort sleep-waking schedule. III. 'Gates' and 'forbidden zones' for sleep. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Relationship of endogenous circadian melatonin and temperature rhythms to self-reported preference for morning or evening activity in young and older people. , 1999, Journal of investigative medicine : the official publication of the American Federation for Clinical Research.

[38]  Gerard A. Kerkhof,et al.  Inter-individual differences in the human circadian system: A review , 1985, Biological Psychology.

[39]  R. Wilkinson Interaction of lack of sleep with knowledge of results, repeated testing, and individual differences. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  J. Emens,et al.  Pretreatment circadian period in free-running blind people may predict the phase angle of entrainment to melatonin , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[41]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations , 1985 .

[42]  D. Dawson,et al.  Salivary Melatonin as a Circadian Phase Marker: Validation and Comparison to Plasma Melatonin , 1997, Journal of biological rhythms.

[43]  Richard E Kronauer,et al.  Comparisons of the Variability of Three Markers of the Human Circadian Pacemaker , 2002, Journal of biological rhythms.

[44]  Mirjam Münch,et al.  Gender and age differences in psychomotor vigilance performance under differential sleep pressure conditions , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[45]  C. Czeisler,et al.  Sex Differences in Phase Angle of Entrainment and Melatonin Amplitude in Humans , 2010, Journal of biological rhythms.

[46]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[47]  Maria L. Thomas,et al.  Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose‐response study , 2003, Journal of sleep research.

[48]  N. Yamada,et al.  HYPERSENSITIVITY OF MELATONIN SUPPRESSION IN RESPONSE TO LIGHT IN PATIENTS WITH DELAYED SLEEP PHASE SYNDROME* , 2001, Chronobiology international.

[49]  María Corsi-Cabrera,et al.  Effect of 38 h of total sleep deprivation on the waking EEG in women: sex differences. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[50]  Greg Maislin,et al.  Dealing with inter-individual differences in the temporal dynamics of fatigue and performance: importance and techniques. , 2004, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[51]  C. Drake,et al.  Effects of rapid versus slow accumulation of eight hours of sleep loss. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[52]  J. Emens,et al.  The circadian basis of winter depression. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  E. F. Colecchia,et al.  Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. , 2003, American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology.

[54]  J. Redman,et al.  Timing of Sleep and Its Relationship with the Endogenous Melatonin Rhythm , 2010, Front. Neur..

[55]  M M Heitkemper,et al.  CIRCADIAN RHYTHMICITY OF CORTISOL AND BODY TEMPERATURE: MORNINGNESS-EVENINGNESS EFFECTS , 2001, Chronobiology international.

[56]  Patricia Mongelia The National Sleep Foundation , 2007 .

[57]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[58]  T. Gislason,et al.  Sleep disturbances in a young adult population: can gender differences be explained by differences in psychological status? , 1997, Sleep.

[59]  W. B. Webb,et al.  Effects of spaced and repeated total sleep deprivation. , 1984, Ergonomics.

[60]  D. Dinges,et al.  Sleep, circadian rhythms, and psychomotor vigilance. , 2005, Clinics in sports medicine.

[61]  B. Phillips,et al.  Adolescent sleep, school start times, and teen motor vehicle crashes. , 2008, Journal of clinical sleep medicine : JCSM : official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

[62]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Circadian pacemaker interferes with sleep onset at specific times each day: role in insomnia. , 1987, The American journal of physiology.

[63]  Makoto Uchiyama,et al.  Circadian rhythm sleep disorders: characteristics and entrainment pathology in delayed sleep phase and non-24-h sleep-wake syndrome. , 2007, Sleep Medicine Reviews.

[64]  C. Landrigan,et al.  Neurobehavioral, health, and safety consequences associated with shift work in safety-sensitive professions , 2009, Current neurology and neuroscience reports.

[65]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Association of intrinsic circadian period with morningness-eveningness, usual wake time, and circadian phase. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[66]  M. Lorr,et al.  Profile of mood states , 1971 .

[67]  J. Broman,et al.  Insufficient sleep in the general population , 1996, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[68]  C. Czeisler,et al.  Improved neurobehavioral performance during the wake maintenance zone. , 2013, Journal of clinical sleep medicine : JCSM : official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

[69]  A. Lewy,et al.  Phase shifting the human circadian clock using melatonin , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[70]  M. Ferrara,et al.  How much sleep do we need? , 2001, Sleep medicine reviews.

[71]  T. Balkin,et al.  PER3 and ADORA2A polymorphisms impact neurobehavioral performance during sleep restriction , 2013, Journal of sleep research.

[72]  B. Bioulac,et al.  Age, performance and sleep deprivation , 2004, Journal of sleep research.