Mightability: A Multi-state Visuo-spatial Reasoning for Human-Robot Interaction

We, the Humans, are capable of estimating various abilities of ourselves and of the person we are interacting with. Visibility and reachability are among two such abilities. Studies in neuroscience and psychology suggest that from the age of 12-15 months children start to understand the occlusion of others line-of-sight and from the age of 3 years they start to develop the ability, termed as perceived reachability for self and for others. As such capabilities evolve in the children, they start showing intuitive and proactive behavior by perceiving various abilities of the human partner.

[1]  P. Rochat Perceived reachability for self and for others by 3- to 5-year-old children and adults. , 1995, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  Reinoud J. Bootsma,et al.  The Effects of Anxiety on Perceiving the Reachability of Passing Objects , 1992 .

[3]  Kazuhito Yokoi,et al.  Reachable Space Generation of A Humanoid Robot Using The Monte Carlo Method , 2006, 2006 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[4]  R. Alami,et al.  Mightability maps: A perceptual level decisional framework for co-operative and competitive human-robot interaction , 2010, 2010 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[5]  A. Campbell,et al.  A longitudinal study of gender-related cognition and behaviour. , 2004, Developmental science.

[6]  Gerd Hirzinger,et al.  Online generation of reachable grasps for dexterous manipulation using a representation of the reachable workspace , 2009, 2009 International Conference on Advanced Robotics.

[7]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' understanding of occlusion of others' line-of-sight: Implications for an emerging theory of mind , 2004 .

[8]  Ricardo Baeza-Yates,et al.  Computer Science 2 , 1994 .

[9]  Andrea Lockerd Thomaz,et al.  Using perspective taking to learn from ambiguous demonstrations , 2006, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[10]  Gerd Hirzinger,et al.  Capturing robot workspace structure: representing robot capabilities , 2007, 2007 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[11]  L. S. Mark,et al.  How Do Task Characteristics Affect the Transitions Between Seated and Standing Reaches? , 2001 .

[12]  J.-P. Laumond,et al.  Move3D: A generic platform for path planning , 2001, Proceedings of the 2001 IEEE International Symposium on Assembly and Task Planning (ISATP2001). Assembly and Disassembly in the Twenty-first Century. (Cat. No.01TH8560).

[13]  Satoshi Kagami,et al.  Efficient prioritized inverse kinematic solutions for redundant manipulators , 2005, 2005 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[14]  J. Gregory Trafton,et al.  Enabling effective human-robot interaction using perspective-taking in robots , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics - Part A: Systems and Humans.

[15]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Scaling affordances for human reach actions. , 2004, Human movement science.

[16]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. , 2004, Developmental science.

[17]  Yiannis Demiris,et al.  Perceptual Perspective Taking and Action Recognition , 2005 .

[18]  Riichiro Tadakuma,et al.  Towards shared attention through geometric reasoning for Human Robot Interaction , 2009, 2009 9th IEEE-RAS International Conference on Humanoid Robots.

[19]  A. Pick,et al.  Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds' joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[20]  M. Turvey,et al.  Visually perceiving what is reachable. , 1989 .

[21]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Comprehension of the Referential Intent of Looking and Pointing Between 12 and 15 Months , 2002 .