Good to be stressed? Improved response inhibition and error processing after acute stress in young and older men

ABSTRACT While aging and stress are both known to affect cognitive functions, little is known on whether and how age modulates stress effects on executive functions and their neural correlates. The current study investigated the effect of acute stress on response inhibition and error processing and their underlying cortical processes in younger and older healthy men, using EEG. Forty‐nine participants (30 young) were stressed with the Trier Social Stress Test (16 young, 9 older) or underwent a friendly control procedure (14 young, 10 older) and subsequently performed a Go/No‐Go task with two levels of task difficulty while performance (reaction time, error rate), stimulus‐locked (N2, P3) and response‐locked (Ne, Pe) ERPs were measured. Previous results on age‐related cognitive deficits were replicated, with slower responses and reduced and delayed N2 and P3 components, as well as reduced Ne and Pe components in older participants. Independent of age, acute stress improved response inhibition, reflected in higher accuracy for compatible trials and enhanced inhibition‐related components (N2, P3 and N2d, P3d of the difference waves No‐Go minus Go), and improved error processing, reflected in enhanced error‐related components (Ne, Pe and Ne_d, Pe_d of the difference waves error minus correct trial). Our findings indicate that acute stress leads to a reallocation of cognitive resources, strengthening inhibition and error processing in young and older healthy men to a similar degree. Neural generators of the analyzed ERPs are mainly part of the salience network, which is upregulated immediately after stress. This offers an explanation as to why response inhibition, in contrast to other executive functions, improves after acute stress. HighlightsAcute stress improves response inhibition accuracy in healthy men.Acute stress enhanced the neural correlates of response inhibition, N2 and P3.Acute stress enhanced error processing reflected by enhanced Ne and Pe amplitudes.This beneficial stress effects may result from activation of the saliency network.These beneficial stress effects were independently of age.

[1]  Lei Zhang,et al.  How Does Emotional Context Modulate Response Inhibition in Alexithymia: Electrophysiological Evidence from an ERP Study , 2012, PloS one.

[2]  B. Bermond,et al.  Psychosocial stress impairs working memory at high loads: An association with cortisol levels and memory retrieval , 2006, Stress.

[3]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[4]  Kazufumi Omura,et al.  Sex differences in neurophysiological responses are modulated by attentional aspects of impulse control , 2015, Brain and Cognition.

[5]  S. Grimm,et al.  Working memory-related frontal theta activity is decreased under acute stress , 2014, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[6]  Oliver T. Wolf,et al.  Psychosocial stress induces working memory impairments in an n-back paradigm , 2008, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[7]  M. Falkenstein Inhibition, conflict and the Nogo-N2 , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[8]  Guangyuan Liu,et al.  Effect of acute psychological stress on response inhibition: An event-related potential study , 2017, Behavioural Brain Research.

[9]  Andrew P Yonelinas,et al.  Acute stress impairs cognitive flexibility in men, not women , 2016, Stress.

[10]  René J. Huster,et al.  The role of the cingulate cortex as neural generator of the N200 and P300 in a tactile response inhibition task , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[11]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Error detection across the adult lifespan: Electrophysiological evidence for age-related deficits , 2017, NeuroImage.

[12]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys , 1994, Psychopharmacology.

[13]  A. Yonelinas,et al.  The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[14]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[15]  Oliver T Wolf,et al.  Cold pressor stress impairs performance on working memory tasks requiring executive functions in healthy young men. , 2009, Behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  M J de Leon,et al.  Cortisol differentially affects memory in young and elderly men. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[18]  R. Barry,et al.  Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[19]  S. Floresco,et al.  Acute stress impairs set-shifting but not reversal learning , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Andrea Kiesel,et al.  The stressed prefrontal cortex and goal-directed behaviour: acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible implementation of task goals , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[22]  W. Helton,et al.  Target predictability, sustained attention, and response inhibition , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[23]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Targets and non-targets in the aging brain: A go/nogo event-related potential study , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[24]  Jens C. Pruessner,et al.  The brain and the stress axis: The neural correlates of cortisol regulation in response to stress , 2009, NeuroImage.

[25]  J. Cerella,et al.  Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[26]  E. Jodo,et al.  Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  J. Howland,et al.  Effects of acute restraint stress on set-shifting and reversal learning in male rats , 2013, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Inhibition-Related ERP Components: Variation with Modality, Age, and Time-on-Task , 2002 .

[29]  S. Dickerson,et al.  Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  Oliver T Wolf,et al.  Sex differences in stress effects on emotional learning , 2017, Journal of neuroscience research.

[32]  A. A. Wijers,et al.  Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[33]  L. Phillips,et al.  Differential Effects of Aging on Executive and Automatic Inhibition , 2008, Developmental neuropsychology.

[34]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[36]  Carl-Walter Kohlmann,et al.  Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der "Positive and Negative Affect Schedule" (PANAS). , 1996 .

[37]  William K. Goodman,et al.  Meta-analytical assessment of the effects of protocol variations on cortisol responses to the Trier Social Stress Test , 2017, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[38]  Lars Schwabe,et al.  Stress and multiple memory systems: from ‘thinking’ to ‘doing’ , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  W. Ziegler The Oxford Handbook Of Event Related Potential Components , 2016 .

[40]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[42]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Alpha 2-adrenergic mechanisms in prefrontal cortex associated with cognitive decline in aged nonhuman primates. , 1985, Science.

[43]  E. Lettich,et al.  Modified nomenclature for the "10%" electrode system. , 1988, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[44]  M. Schwarz,et al.  Response Monitoring in De Novo Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2009, PloS one.

[45]  Mandy J. Maguire,et al.  Effects of age on cognitive control during semantic categorization , 2015, Behavioural Brain Research.

[46]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[47]  A. Fallgatter,et al.  The NoGo-anteriorization as a neurophysiological standard-index for cognitive response control. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[48]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  An electrophysiological study of response conflict processing across the lifespan: Assessing the roles of conflict monitoring, cue utilization, response anticipation, and response suppression , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Pei-Luen Patrick Rau,et al.  The detrimental effect of acute stress on response inhibition when exposed to acute stress: an event-related potential analysis , 2017, Neuroreport.

[50]  E. Lettich,et al.  Modified Nomenclature for the “10%” Electrode System1 , 1988 .

[51]  G. Band,et al.  Inhibitory motor control in stop paradigms: review and reinterpretation of neural mechanisms. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[52]  Julian C. L. Lai Psychosocial Stress and Salivary Cortisol in Older People: A Brief Review , 2014 .

[53]  R. Yerkes,et al.  The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation , 1908 .

[54]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Aging and Error Processing: Age Related Increase in the Variability of the Error-Negativity Is Not Accompanied by Increase in Response Variability , 2011, PloS one.

[55]  C. Kirschbaum,et al.  Salivary alpha-amylase stress reactivity across different age groups. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[56]  Anne Bonnefond,et al.  Investigating sustained attention ability in the elderly by using two different approaches: inhibiting ongoing behavior versus responding on rare occasions. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[57]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  The Effects of Aging on Conflict Detection , 2013, PloS one.

[58]  C. Beckmann,et al.  How the brain connects in response to acute stress: A review at the human brain systems level , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[59]  Oliver T Wolf,et al.  Influence of acute stress on response inhibition in healthy men: An ERP study. , 2017, Psychophysiology.

[60]  Florin C. Popescu,et al.  Single-subject prediction of response inhibition behavior by event-related potentials. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  B. Porjesz,et al.  Neurocognitive deficits in male alcoholics: An ERP/sLORETA analysis of the N2 component in an equal probability Go/NoGo task , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[62]  Lars Schwabe,et al.  Stress-induced enhancement of response inhibition depends on mineralocorticoid receptor activation , 2013, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[63]  C. Kirschbaum,et al.  The 'Trier Social Stress Test'--a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.

[64]  A meta-analysis of cortisol response to challenge in human aging: importance of gender , 2005, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[65]  Arno Villringer,et al.  Gender Influences on Brain Responses to Errors and Post-Error Adjustments , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[66]  N. Yeung,et al.  On the ERN and the significance of errors. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[67]  O. Wolf Effects of Stress on Memory: Relevance for Human Aging , 2015 .

[68]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Effect of Aging on ERP Components of Cognitive Control , 2016, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[69]  M. Hughes,et al.  The spatial and temporal dynamics of anticipatory preparation and response inhibition in task-switching , 2010, NeuroImage.

[70]  Andreas Mueller,et al.  Dissociating action inhibition, conflict monitoring and sensory mismatch into independent components of event related potentials in GO/NOGO task , 2011, NeuroImage.

[71]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[72]  A. Marks,et al.  Practice-Related Effects in a Go-Nogo Task , 2007, Perceptual and motor skills.

[73]  A. Arnsten Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[74]  G. Fernández,et al.  Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors , 2014, Trends in Neurosciences.

[75]  B. Kudielka,et al.  HPA axis responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and children: impact of age and gender , 2004, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[76]  Michael M. Plichta,et al.  Sequential inhibitory control processes assessed through simultaneous EEG–fMRI , 2014, NeuroImage.

[77]  O. Wolf,et al.  A friendly version of the Trier Social Stress Test does not activate the HPA axis in healthy men and women , 2013, Stress.

[78]  A. Salvador,et al.  Acute stress impairs recall after interference in older people, but not in young people , 2014, Hormones and Behavior.