Performance monitoring across the lifespan: Still maturing post-conflict regulation in children and declining task-set monitoring in older adults

[1]  M. Ullsperger,et al.  Neural mechanisms and temporal dynamics of performance monitoring , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Cognitive aging: is there a dark side to environmental support? , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  G. Jackson,et al.  Learning and altering behaviours by reinforcement: Neurocognitive differences between children and adults , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Lower theta inter-trial phase coherence during performance monitoring is related to higher reaction time variability: A lifespan study , 2013, NeuroImage.

[5]  Peter J. Molfese,et al.  A Developmental Study of the Feedback-Related Negativity From 10–17 Years: Age and Sex Effects for Reward Versus Non-Reward , 2013, Developmental neuropsychology.

[6]  Shu-Chen Li Neuromodulation and developmental contextual influences on neural and cognitive plasticity across the lifespan , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[7]  Nicola K. Ferdinand,et al.  Age-related changes in processing positive and negative feedback: Is there a positivity effect for older adults? , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[8]  Markus Ullsperger,et al.  Real and Fictive Outcomes Are Processed Differently but Converge on a Common Adaptive Mechanism , 2013, Neuron.

[9]  M. Maroun,et al.  Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2013, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[10]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Normative shifts of cortical mechanisms of encoding contribute to adult age differences in visual–spatial working memory , 2013, NeuroImage.

[11]  Deborah Talmi,et al.  The Feedback-Related Negativity Signals Salience Prediction Errors, Not Reward Prediction Errors , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Effects of PPP1R1B (DARPP-32) Polymorphism on Feedback-Related Brain Potentials Across the Life Span , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[13]  R. E. Martin Event-related potential studies of outcome processing and feedback-guided learning , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  L. Nystrom,et al.  Role of prefrontal cortex and the midbrain dopamine system in working memory updating , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Learning from experience: Event-related potential correlates of reward processing, neural adaptation, and behavioral choice , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  Viktor Müller,et al.  Neural Synchrony during Response Production and Inhibition , 2012, PloS one.

[17]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Aging and Inhibitory Control of Action: Cortico-Subthalamic Connection Strength Predicts Stopping Performance , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Sergeant,et al.  Developmental Trajectories of Neural Mechanisms Supporting Conflict and Error Processing in Middle Childhood , 2012, Developmental neuropsychology.

[20]  Brad E. Sheese,et al.  Control networks and neuromodulators of early development. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[21]  Sidney J. Segalowitz,et al.  Performance monitoring and the medial prefrontal cortex: a review of individual differences and context effects as a window on self-regulation , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[22]  D. Hämmerer,et al.  Dopaminergic and prefrontal contributions to reward-based learning and outcome monitoring during child development and aging. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[23]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Inter-individual performance differences in younger and older adults differentially relate to amplitude modulations and phase stability of oscillations controlling working memory contents , 2012, NeuroImage.

[24]  J. Grose-Fifer,et al.  The feedback-related negativity (FRN) in adolescents. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[25]  T. Endrass,et al.  Speeding up older adults: Age-effects on error processing in speed and accuracy conditions , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[26]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Amplitude modulations and inter-trial phase stability of alpha-oscillations differentially reflect working memory constraints across the lifespan , 2012, NeuroImage.

[27]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in the Elderly Brain: A Structural Model-Based Approach , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Age, sex and individual differences in punishment sensitivity: factors influencing the feedback-related negativity. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[29]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  On the role of fronto-striatal neural synchronization processes for response inhibition—Evidence from ERP phase-synchronization analyses in pre-manifest Huntington's disease gene mutation carriers , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Neuromodulation of reward‐based learning and decision making in human aging , 2011, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Seth A. Herd,et al.  A unified framework for inhibitory control , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  G. Potts Impact of reward and punishment motivation on behavior monitoring as indexed by the error-related negativity. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[33]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Load Modulation of BOLD Response and Connectivity Predicts Working Memory Performance in Younger and Older Adults , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Reisa A. Sperling,et al.  Failure to Modulate Attentional Control in Advanced Aging Linked to White Matter Pathology , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  M. Ullsperger,et al.  Post-Error Adjustments , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[37]  C. Basar-Eroglu,et al.  Event-related delta and theta brain oscillations reflect age-related changes in both a general and a specific neuronal inhibitory mechanism , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[38]  A. Gazzaley,et al.  Influence of early attentional modulation on working memory , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  In Brief , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  I. Daum,et al.  Processing of expected and unexpected monetary performance outcomes in healthy older subjects. , 2011, Behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  Viktor Müller,et al.  Life Span Differences in Electrophysiological Correlates of Monitoring Gains and Losses during Probabilistic Reinforcement Learning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  N. Yeung,et al.  Dissociable correlates of response conflict and error awareness in error-related brain activity , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  T. Endrass,et al.  Aging, probabilistic learning and performance monitoring , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[44]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Age related strategic differences in processing irrelevant information , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[45]  Anna Weinberg,et al.  Increased error-related brain activity in generalized anxiety disorder , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[46]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Strengthening of Top-Down Frontal Cognitive Control Networks Underlying the Development of Inhibitory Control: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Effective Connectivity Study , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  N. Yeung,et al.  Decision Processes in Human Performance Monitoring , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Temporal dynamics of prediction error processing during reward-based decision making , 2010, NeuroImage.

[49]  William H. Alexander,et al.  Computational Models of Performance Monitoring and Cognitive Control , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[50]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  Post-Error Behavioral Adjustments Are Facilitated by Activation and Suppression of Task-Relevant and Task-Irrelevant Information Processing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  An electrophysiological study of response conflict processing across the lifespan: Assessing the roles of conflict monitoring, cue utilization, response anticipation, and response suppression , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Independent component analysis of erroneous and correct responses suggests online response control , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[53]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  Reliable differences in brain activity between young and old adults: A quantitative meta-analysis across multiple cognitive domains , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[54]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Age-related changes in grey and white matter structure throughout adulthood , 2010, NeuroImage.

[55]  Borís Burle,et al.  Rostral Cingulate Zone and correct response monitoring: ICA and source localization evidences for the unicity of correct- and error-negativities , 2010, NeuroImage.

[56]  T. White,et al.  Developmental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: Behavioral implications and issues in assessment , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[57]  Michelle K. Jetha,et al.  Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: A review , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[58]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  N. De Stefano,et al.  Longitudinal changes in grey and white matter during adolescence , 2010, NeuroImage.

[60]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Performance level modulates adult age differences in brain activation during spatial working memory , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[61]  S. Nieuwenhuis,et al.  Dissociating Response Conflict and Error Likelihood in Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  M. Ullsperger,et al.  Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[63]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Lifespan differences in cortical dynamics of auditory perception. , 2009, Developmental science.

[64]  Sidney J. Segalowitz,et al.  Individual differences and developmental change in the ERN response: implications for models of ACC function , 2009, Psychological research.

[65]  Jutta Kray,et al.  Developmental differences in learning and error processing: evidence from ERPs. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[66]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Functional Brain Networks Develop from a “Local to Distributed” Organization , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[67]  A. Compte,et al.  Mechanism for top-down control of working memory capacity , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[69]  B. Eppinger,et al.  Better or worse than expected? Aging, learning, and the ERN , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  B. Hommel,et al.  Lifespan development of stimulus-response conflict cost: similarities and differences between maturation and senescence , 2008, Psychological research.

[71]  M. E. Wheeler,et al.  Maturational changes in anterior cingulate and frontoparietal recruitment support the development of error processing and inhibitory control. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[72]  William J. Tays,et al.  Aging and electrocortical response to error feedback during a spatial learning task. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[73]  Kristine B Walhovd,et al.  P300 amplitude age reductions are not caused by latency jitter. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[74]  T. Endrass,et al.  Changes of performance monitoring with learning in older and younger adults. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[75]  T. Endrass,et al.  Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous reactions , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[76]  T. Braver,et al.  Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[77]  T. C. Lorsbach,et al.  Context Processing and Cognitive Control in Children and Young Adults , 2008, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[78]  Martin E. Maier,et al.  Modeling behavioral measures of error detection in choice tasks: response monitoring versus conflict monitoring. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[79]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  ERP—Correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm , 2008, Brain Research.

[80]  J. Meere,et al.  Developmental changes in error monitoring: An event-related potential study , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[82]  S. Petersen,et al.  Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[83]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[84]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain , 2007, NeuroImage.

[85]  Eun Young Kim,et al.  Error-Related Negativity in a Visual Go/No-Go Task: Children vs. Adults , 2007, Developmental neuropsychology.

[86]  L. Nyberg,et al.  The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: Current status and future prospects , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[87]  P. Zelazo,et al.  Neural correlates of cognitive control in childhood and adolescence: Disentangling the contributions of age and executive function , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[88]  M. Brammer,et al.  Progressive increase of frontostriatal brain activation from childhood to adulthood during event‐related tasks of cognitive control , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[89]  E. Crone,et al.  Brain Regions Mediating Flexible Rule Use during Development , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[90]  John G. Kerns,et al.  Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an FMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[91]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  Outsourcing cognitive control to the environment: Adult age differences in the use of task cues , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[92]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex activity can be independent of response conflict in Stroop-like tasks , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[93]  Charles H. Hillman,et al.  Age and physical activity influences on action monitoring during task switching , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[94]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Error-related electrocortical responses in 10-year-old children and young adults. , 2006, Developmental science.

[95]  J. Kray Task-set switching under cue-based versus memory-based switching conditions in younger and older adults , 2006, Brain Research.

[96]  T. Robbins,et al.  Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: Comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[97]  L. Jonkman,et al.  The development of preparation, conflict monitoring and inhibition from early childhood to young adulthood; a Go/Nogo ERP study , 2006, Brain Research.

[98]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Does the error negativity reflect the degree of response conflict? , 2006, Brain Research.

[99]  Allen Azizian,et al.  Beware misleading cues: perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[100]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring , 2006, Psychological science.

[101]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[102]  Cheryl L. Dahle,et al.  Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[103]  F. Vargha-Khadem,et al.  Maturation of action monitoring from adolescence to adulthood: an ERP study. , 2005, Developmental science.

[104]  Jeffrey W. Cooney,et al.  Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[105]  Paige E. Scalf,et al.  The implications of cortical recruitment and brain morphology for individual differences in inhibitory function in aging humans. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[106]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Aging Neuromodulation Impairs Associative Binding , 2005, Psychological science.

[107]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[108]  C. Liston,et al.  Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[109]  A. Arnsten,et al.  Neurobiology of Executive Functions: Catecholamine Influences on Prefrontal Cortical Functions , 2004, Biological Psychiatry.

[110]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[111]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Computational roles for dopamine in behavioural control , 2004, Nature.

[112]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[113]  E. Crone,et al.  Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecendents and consequences of decision making. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[114]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Source localization (LORETA) of the error-related-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe). , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[115]  C. Carter,et al.  ERP Correlates of Action Monitoring in Adolescence , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[116]  Sidney J Segalowitz,et al.  Development of Response-Monitoring ERPs in 7- to 25-Year-Olds , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[117]  J. Sweeney,et al.  The Emergence of Collaborative Brain Function: fMRI Studies of the Development of Response Inhibition , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[118]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[119]  B. Burle,et al.  Electroencephalographic nogo potentials in a no-movement context: the case of motor imagery in humans , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[120]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Context dependence of the event-related brain potential associated with reward and punishment. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[121]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[122]  L. Jonkman,et al.  Developmental differences in behavioral and event-related brain responses associated with response preparation and inhibition in a go/nogo task. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[123]  S. Resnick,et al.  Longitudinal Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Older Adults: A Shrinking Brain , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[124]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Mapping cortical change across the human life span , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[125]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[126]  A. Turken,et al.  Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[127]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[128]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[129]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[130]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  A computational account of altered error processing in older age: Dopamine and the error-related negativity , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[131]  S. Langenecker,et al.  Differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory control across the adult life span. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[132]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[133]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Immature Frontal Lobe Contributions to Cognitive Control in Children Evidence from fMRI , 2002, Neuron.

[134]  W. Gehring,et al.  Functions of the Medial Frontal Cortex in the Processing of Conflict and Errors , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[135]  S. Sikström,et al.  Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[136]  N. Cohen,et al.  General and task-specific frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: A fMRI investigation of task-switching , 2001, Neuroreport.

[137]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[138]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Changes of error-related ERPs with age , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[139]  U. Mayr Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[140]  E. Miller,et al.  The prefontral cortex and cognitive control , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[141]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[142]  M. Coles,et al.  Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[143]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[144]  G. Band,et al.  Age effects on response monitoring in a mental-rotation task , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[145]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[146]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Cognition and control in schizophrenia: a computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[147]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[148]  Jean-Claude Baron,et al.  Emission tomography contribution to clinical neurology , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[149]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[150]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[151]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Sources of interference from irrelevant information: a developmental study. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[152]  M. Coles,et al.  "Where did I go wrong?" A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[153]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[154]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[155]  Neil Brewer,et al.  Developmental changes in processing speed: Influence of speed-accuracy regulation , 1989 .

[156]  Jutta Kray,et al.  To Choose or to Avoid: Age Differences in Learning from Positive and Negative Feedback , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[157]  S. Haber,et al.  The Reward Circuit: Linking Primate Anatomy and Human Imaging , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[158]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of flexible cognitive control in humans , 2010 .

[159]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Developmental differences in error-related ERPs in middle- to late-adolescent males. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[160]  R. Barry,et al.  Development of Inhibitory Processing During the Go/NoGo Task: A Behavioral and Event-Related Potenti , 2005 .

[161]  A. Diamond,et al.  Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. , 2004, The American journal of psychiatry.

[162]  Sidney J Segalowitz,et al.  The error-related negativity as a state and trait measure: motivation, personality, and ERPs in response to errors. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[163]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Context processing in older adults: evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[164]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[165]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dopamine, cognitive control, and schizophrenia: the gating model. , 1999, Progress in brain research.

[166]  M. Klein Cognitive Changes due to Aging and Fatigue as revealed in the Electrical Brain Activity , 1999 .

[167]  J L Rapoport,et al.  The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic and controlled processes: a developmental neuroanatomical study. , 1997, Developmental psychobiology.

[168]  G. Smith,et al.  Slowness and age: speed-accuracy mechanisms. , 1995, Psychology and aging.