Activation of the intermediate sum in intentional and automatic calculations

Most research investigating how the cognitive system deals with arithmetic has focused on the processing of two addends. Arithmetic that involves more addends has specific cognitive demands such as the need to compute and hold the intermediate sum. This study examines the intermediate sums activations in intentional and automatic calculations. Experiment 1 included addition problems containing three operands. Participants were asked to calculate the sum and to remember the digits that appeared in the problem. The results revealed an interference effect in which it was hard to identify that the digit representing the intermediate sum was not actually one of the operands. Experiment 2, further examined if the intermediate sum is activated automatically when a task does not require calculation. Here, participants were presented with a prime of an addition problem followed by a target number. The task was to determine whether the target number is odd or even, while ignoring the addition problem in the prime. The results suggested that the intermediate sum of the addition problem in the prime was activated automatically and facilitated the target. Overall, the implications of those findings in the context of theories that relate to cognitive mathematical calculation is further discussed.

[1]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[2]  K. Wynn,et al.  Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.

[3]  J. Coney,et al.  Simple arithmetic processing: Individual differences in automaticity , 2007 .

[4]  Nir Levy,et al.  Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  M. Ashcraft Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.

[6]  D. Ariely Seeing Sets: Representation by Statistical Properties , 2001, Psychological science.

[7]  J. LeFevre,et al.  The extension of the interference effect to multiplication. , 1996, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[8]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach☆ , 1982 .

[9]  A. Oliva,et al.  The Representation of Simple Ensemble Visual Features Outside the Focus of Attention , 2008, Psychological science.

[10]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  Cognitive Architecture , 2003 .

[11]  A. Treisman,et al.  Statistical processing: computing the average size in perceptual groups , 2005, Vision Research.

[12]  J. Coney,et al.  Simple arithmetic processing: the question of automaticity. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[13]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The cognitive architecture for chaining of two mental operations , 2009, Cognition.

[14]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[15]  Patrick Lemaire,et al.  Different brain mechanisms mediate two strategies in arithmetic: evidence from Event-Related brain Potentials , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  G. Hitch The role of short-term working memory in mental arithmetic , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Rapid parallel semantic processing of numbers without awareness , 2011, Cognition.

[18]  K. Tamura,et al.  Metabolic engineering of plant alkaloid biosynthesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A , 2001 .

[19]  Fürst Aj,et al.  The role of working memory in mental arithmetic. , 1998 .

[20]  Jo-Anne LeFevre,et al.  Individual differences in the obligatory activation of addition facts , 1994, Memory & cognition.