Sodium Amylobarbitone and Responses to Nonreward*
暂无分享,去创建一个
J. Gray | J. Feldon | H. de Wit | N. McNaughton | A. Guillamón | J. Feldon, | J. Gray
[1] D. James. Barbiturate effects on nonreward are time dependent. , 1978, Behavioral biology.
[2] Roger L. Mellgren,et al. Patterning, partial reinforcement, and N-length effects at spaced trials as a function of reinstatement of retrieval cues , 1977 .
[3] H. L. Petri,et al. Relationship between hippocampal EEG, novelty, and frustration in the rat. , 1974, Behavioral biology.
[4] E. Capaldi,et al. Partial reward training level and reward magnitude: Effects on acquisition and extinction , 1974 .
[5] P. Wong,et al. Short-term and long-term factors in extinction and durable persistence , 1971 .
[6] E. Capaldi,et al. Trial spacing and emotionality in the rat. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[7] N. Mackintosh,et al. Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning , 1971 .
[8] E. Capaldi,et al. Amobarbital and the partial reinforcement effects in rats: isolating frustrative control over instrumental responding. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[9] J. Gray,et al. Sodium amylobarbitone, the partial reinforcement extinction effect, and the frustration effect in the double runway. , 1971, Neuropharmacology.
[10] J. Gray,et al. Frequency-Specific Relation between Hippocampal Theta Rhythm, Behavior, and Amobarbital Action , 1970, Science.
[11] J. Gray,et al. Sodium amobarbital and effects of frustrative nonreward. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[12] J. Ison,et al. Interaction of amobarbital sodium and reinforcement schedule in determining resistance to extinction of an instrumental running response. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[13] A. Amsel,et al. Number of food pellets, goal approaches and the partial reinforcement effect after minimal acquisition. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[14] J. Ison,et al. Amobarbital Sodium and the Effects of Reward and Nonreward in the Amsel Double Runway , 1967, Psychological reports.
[15] H. Ludvigson. A preliminary investigation of the effects of sodium amytal, prior reward in G1 and activity level on the FE , 1967 .
[16] J. Ison,et al. Amobarbital sodium and instrumental performance changes following reward reduction , 1967 .
[17] R. Wilton,et al. Extinction Performance as a Function of Reinforcement Magnitude and Number of Training Trials , 1967, Psychological reports.
[18] H. Terrace,et al. Behavioral contrast and the peak shift: effects of extended discrimination training. , 1966, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[19] K. Spence,et al. Effects of intertrial reinforcement on resistance to extinction following extended training. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.
[20] A. R. Wagner,et al. RESISTANCE TO PUNISHMENT AND EXTINCTION FOLLOWING TRAINING WITH SHOCK OR NONREINFORCEMENT. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.
[21] R. Stretch,et al. Effects of Amobarbital on Extinction of an Instrumental Response in Rats , 1964, Nature.
[22] A. R. Wagner. Sodium amytal and partially reinforced runway performance. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.
[23] E. Capaldi,et al. Temporal properties of reinforcement aftereffects. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.
[24] N. Miller,et al. Effects of alcohol and amobarbital on performance inhibited by experimental extinction. , 1962, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[25] A. Amsel. Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: some recent history and a theoretical extension. , 1962, Psychological review.
[26] A. R. Wagner,et al. Effects of amount and percentage of reinforcement and number of acquisition trials on conditioning and extinction. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.
[27] S. Weinstock. Acquisition and extinction of a partially reinforced running response at a 24-hour intertrial interval. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.
[28] N. Miller,et al. A positive relationship between reinforcement and resistance to extinction produced by removing a source of confusion from a technique that had produced opposite results. , 1947, Journal of experimental psychology.
[29] J. Gray. Drug Effects on Fear and Frustration: Possible Limbic Site of Action of Minor Tranquilizers , 1977 .
[30] A. Fernández. Alternancia simple utilizando agua como refuerzo: eliminación de estímulos exteroceptivos , 1977 .
[31] N. Mackintosh. The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .
[32] E. Capaldi,et al. Amytal and the small trial partial reinforcement effect: Stimulus properties of early trial nonrewards , 1971 .
[33] M. Bitterman,et al. Spaced-trials partial reinforcement effect as a function of contrast. , 1969 .
[34] E. Capaldi. A Sequential Hypothesis of Instrumental Learning1 , 1967 .
[35] A. Amsel,et al. Partial Reinforcement Effects On Vigor and Persistence: Advances in Frustration Theory Derived from a Variety of Within-Subjects Experiments1 , 1967 .
[36] K. Spence,et al. Intertrial reinforcement and the partial reinforcement effect as a function of number of training trials , 1965 .
[37] E. Capaldi,et al. Effect of intertrial reinforcement on the aftereffect of nonreinforcement and resistance to extinction. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.
[38] J. Ison. Experimental extinction as a function of number of reinforcements. , 1962 .
[39] K. P. Goodrich. Performance in different segments of an instrumental response chain as a function of reinforcement schedule. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[40] D. Haggard. Acquisition of a simple running response as a function of partial and continuous schedules of reinforcement , 1959 .