Revisiting psychophysical work on the quantitative and qualitative odour properties of simple odour mixtures: a flavour chemistry view. Part 1: intensity and detectability. A review.

The different psychophysical studies carried out on the quantitative properties of binary mixtures are reviewed. Results from 520 mixtures have been reprocessed and plotted together. The most likely outcome of a binary mixture of two different odorants at supra-threshold levels is a significant reduction of its odour intensity not only with respect to the total addition of intensities, but also to what could be expected from an auto-addition following Stevens's law. Half of the mixtures behave asymmetrically, so that one of the odorants can reduce the overall odour intensity of the mixture more efficiently than the other. This is most frequent when there is a marked difference in the hedonic character of the odorants, and the least pleasant odorant plays the role of suppressor. Such suppression can be quantitatively relevant. The addition of the third, fourth or fifth isointense constituent do not cause any further intensity increase, which indicates the existence of strong signal compression mechanisms at supra-receptor level. Synergism is highly infrequent and may be linked to the appearance of new odour concepts. In the case of mixtures of sub-threshold odorants, the oldest reports are based on the odour activity value concept, and, in general, show the existence of a certain degree of cooperation between the components of the mixture. The most recent research makes use of detectability functions and probabilistic considerations and has demonstrated that cooperation can be categorized as hypoaddition, perfect addition (independence of response) or even synergy. Hypoaddition is most frequent at higher concentrations. Some reports suggest that hypoaddition is related to the structural similarity but results are not conclusive. Other reports demonstrate that under or peri-threshold odorants are able to introduce significant qualitative and quantitative changes on supra-threshold odour solutions. So far it is not feasible to assess when and why this enhancing effect takes place. It can be hypothesized that the answer will require an integral treatment of quality and intensity. Copyright © 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

[1]  J E Frijters,et al.  Psychophysical Models for Mixtures of Tastants and Mixtures of Odorants , 1987, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  S. Nicklaus,et al.  Perceptual interactions between fruity and woody notes of wine , 2004 .

[3]  W. Grosch Evaluation of the key odorants of foods by dilution experiments, aroma models and omission. , 2001, Chemical senses.

[4]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Analysis, occurrence and potential sensory significance of aliphatic aldehydes in white wines. , 2011, Food chemistry.

[5]  D. G. Laing,et al.  The capacity of humans to identify odors in mixtures , 1989, Physiology & Behavior.

[6]  William S. Cain,et al.  ODOR INTENSITY: MIXTURES AND MASKING , 1975 .

[7]  H. Hudnell,et al.  Agonistic sensory effects of airborne chemicals in mixtures: Odor, nasal pungency, and eye irritation , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  Howard R. Moskowitz,et al.  Flavor Chemical Mixtures—A Psychophysical Analysis , 1977 .

[9]  E. Keith,et al.  Determination of Flavor Threshold Levels and Sub‐Threshold, Additive, and Concentration Effects , 1968 .

[10]  S. S. Stevens The Psychophysics of Sensory Function. , 1960 .

[11]  T Lindvall,et al.  A quantitative principle of perceived intensity summation in odor mixtures. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  Paul Laffort,et al.  An alternative model of olfactory quantitative interaction in binary mixtures , 1979 .

[13]  M. Chastrette,et al.  L'intensité de l'odeur des mélanges binaires de composés odorants , 2002 .

[14]  M. Olsson,et al.  Odor-intensity interaction in binary and ternary mixtures , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  B. Berglund,et al.  Odor-intensity interaction in binary mixtures , 1993 .

[16]  D. G. Laing,et al.  Natural Sniffing Gives Optimum Odour Perception for Humans , 1983, Perception.

[17]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Quality and aromatic sensory descriptors (mainly fresh and dry fruit character) of Spanish red wines can be predicted from their aroma-active chemical composition. , 2011, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.

[18]  S. Nicklaus,et al.  Perception of wine fruity and woody notes: influence of peri-threshold odorants , 2005 .

[19]  D. G. Laing,et al.  Perception of components in binary odour mixtures , 1983 .

[20]  Michael H. Abraham,et al.  Concentration-detection functions for the odor of homologous n-acetate esters , 2008, Physiology & Behavior.

[21]  William S. Cain,et al.  Detection thresholds for an olfactory mixture and its three constituent compounds , 1993 .

[22]  M. Abraham,et al.  Odor detection by humans of lineal aliphatic aldehydes and helional as gauged by dose-response functions. , 2010, Chemical senses.

[23]  S. S. Stevens On the psychophysical law. , 1957, Psychological review.

[24]  Michelle Gallagher,et al.  Synergistic mixture interactions in detection of perithreshold odors by humans. , 2008, Chemical senses.

[25]  Concentration Effect on Odor Addition or Synergism in Mixtures of Methyl Sulfide and Tomato Juice , 1969 .

[26]  Implications of power law exponent in synergy and inhibition of olfactory mixtures , 1989 .

[27]  T. Thomas-Danguin,et al.  Just noticeable differences in component concentrations modify the odor quality of a blending mixture. , 2008, Chemical senses.

[28]  B. Berglund,et al.  QUANTITATIVE AND QUALITATIVE ANALYSIS OF INDUSTRIAL ODORS WITH HUMAN OBSERVERS * , 1974, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[29]  D. G. Laing,et al.  The analysis of odor mixtures by humans: evidence for a configurational process , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.

[30]  M. Abraham,et al.  Olfactory detectability of homologous n-alkylbenzenes as reflected by concentration-detection functions in humans , 2009, Neuroscience.

[31]  M. Laska,et al.  A comparison of the detection thresholds of odour mixtures and their components , 1991 .

[32]  M. Abraham,et al.  Structure–activity relationships on the odor detectability of homologous carboxylic acids by humans , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  T. Peppard,et al.  Characterization of Tequila Flavor by Instrumental and Sensory Analysis , 1996 .

[34]  B. Green,et al.  Derivation and evaluation of a semantic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties , 1993 .

[35]  V. Ferreira,et al.  An assessment of the role played by some oxidation-related aldehydes in wine aroma. , 2007, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.

[36]  M. Abraham,et al.  Olfactory psychometric functions for homologous 2-ketones , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[37]  G. Preti,et al.  Human odor detection of homologous carboxylic acids and their binary mixtures. , 2007, Chemical senses.

[38]  R. Buttery,et al.  Additive Effect of Sub-Threshold Concentrations of Some Organic Compounds Associated with Food Aromas , 1963, Nature.

[39]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Characterization of the aroma of a wine from maccabeo. Key role played by compounds with low odor activity values. , 2004, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.

[40]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Analysis, occurrence, and potential sensory significance of five polyfunctional mercaptans in white wines. , 2010, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.

[41]  Paul Laffort,et al.  Several models of suprathreshold quantitative olfactory interaction in humans applied to binary, ternary and quaternary mixtures , 1982 .

[42]  William S Cain,et al.  Chemosensory Detectability of 1-Butanol and 2-Heptanone Singly and in Binary Mixtures , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[43]  M Chastrette,et al.  Modelling the human olfactory stimulus-response function. , 1998, Chemical senses.

[44]  R. Gregson Qualitative and aqualitative intensity components of odour mixtures , 1986 .

[45]  D. G. Laing,et al.  Perceptual characteristics of binary, trinary, and quaternary odor mixtures consisting of unpleasant constituents , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[46]  M. Abraham,et al.  Odor detection of single chemicals and binary mixtures , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[47]  W S Cain,et al.  Comparison of models of odor interaction. , 1995, Chemical senses.

[48]  R. Buttery,et al.  Odour thresholds of some organic compounds associated with food flavours , 1963 .

[49]  F. N. Jones,et al.  ON THE INTENSITY OF ODOR MIXTURES * , 1964, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[50]  Paul Laffort,et al.  SELECTED AND STANDARDIZED VALUES OF SUPRATHRESHOLD ODOR INTENSITIES FOR 110 SUBSTANCES , 1975 .

[51]  H. Schifferstein,et al.  The influence of olfactory concept on the probability of detecting sub- and peri-threshold components in a mixture of odorants. , 2001, Chemical senses.

[52]  M. Olsson An interaction model for odor quality and intensity , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[53]  D. G. Laing,et al.  Quality and intensity of binary odor mixtures , 1984, Physiology & Behavior.

[54]  Michelle Gallagher,et al.  Psychometric functions for ternary odor mixtures and their unmixed components. , 2009, Chemical senses.

[55]  Michelle Gallagher,et al.  The Impact of Subthreshold Carboxylic Acids on the Odor Intensity of Suprathreshold Flavor Compounds , 2008 .

[56]  B Cowart,et al.  Evaluating the 'Labeled Magnitude Scale' for measuring sensations of taste and smell. , 1996, Chemical senses.

[57]  Yuriko Ito,et al.  Sensory evaluation of the synergism among odorants present in concentrations below their odor threshold in a Chinese jasmine green tea infusion. , 2005, Molecular nutrition & food research.

[58]  W S Cain,et al.  SCOPE AND EVALUATION OF ODOR COUNTERACTION AND MASKING * , 1974, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[59]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Release and formation of varietal aroma compounds during alcoholic fermentation from nonfloral grape odorless flavor precursors fractions. , 2007, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.

[60]  William S Cain,et al.  Dose-addition of individual odorants in the odor detection of binary mixtures , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[61]  Eva Campo,et al.  Prediction of the wine sensory properties related to grape variety from dynamic-headspace gas chromatography-olfactometry data. , 2005, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.

[62]  George Preti,et al.  Odor detection of mixtures of homologous carboxylic acids and coffee aroma compounds by humans. , 2009, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.