The Stochastic Early Reaction, Inhibition, and Late Action (SERIA) Model for Antisaccades
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Gary Feng,et al. Is there a common control mechanism for anti-saccades and reading eye movements? Evidence from distributional analyses , 2012, Vision Research.
[2] O. Tzeng,et al. Trial type probability modulates the cost of antisaccades. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[3] F. M. D. P. Martín,et al. A Theory of Reaction Time Distributions , 2009 .
[4] G. Logan,et al. Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.
[5] Jennifer E McDowell,et al. Effects of preparation time and trial type probability on performance of anti- and pro-saccades. , 2016, Acta psychologica.
[6] Cristina Massen,et al. Parallel programming of exogenous and endogenous components in the antisaccade task , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[7] Monique Ernst,et al. Anxiety, a benefit and detriment to cognition: Behavioral and magnetoencephalographic evidence from a mixed-saccade task , 2012, Brain and Cognition.
[8] Thomas V. Wiecki,et al. A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. , 2011, Psychological review.
[9] Vassilis Cutsuridis,et al. Neural competition via lateral inhibition between decision processes and not a STOP signal accounts for the antisaccade performance in healthy and schizophrenia subjects , 2015, Front. Neurosci..
[10] T. Patterson,et al. The optimum addition of points to quadrature formulae. , 1968 .
[11] Douglas P. Munoz,et al. Preparatory neural networks are impaired in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during the antisaccade task , 2012, NeuroImage: Clinical.
[12] Brian C Coe,et al. Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti-saccade task , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[13] Karl J. Friston,et al. Bayesian model selection for group studies , 2009, NeuroImage.
[14] Juan Fernandez-Ruiz,et al. Cognitive Deterioration and Functional Compensation in ALS Measured with fMRI Using an Inhibitory Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] Karl J. Friston,et al. Bayesian model selection for group studies — Revisited , 2014, NeuroImage.
[16] C. Mathys,et al. Computational approaches to psychiatry , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[17] Imran Noorani,et al. Movement suppression: brain mechanisms for stopping and stillness , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[18] Scott D. Brown,et al. Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[19] Michael F. Green,et al. Robust differences in antisaccade performance exist between COGS schizophrenia cases and controls regardless of recruitment strategies , 2015, Schizophrenia Research.
[20] Eduardo A. Aponte,et al. Computational models of eye movements and their application to schizophrenia , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[21] J. Schall,et al. Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] R. Schmidt,et al. A Pause-then-Cancel model of stopping: evidence from basal ganglia neurophysiology , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[23] Joachim M. Buhmann,et al. Generative Embedding for Model-Based Classification of fMRI Data , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[24] Mark A. Girolami,et al. Estimating Bayes factors via thermodynamic integration and population MCMC , 2009, Comput. Stat. Data Anal..
[25] R. H. S. Carpenter,et al. Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.
[26] Jay A. Edelman,et al. Antisaccade velocity, but not latency, results from a lack of saccade visual guidance , 2006, Vision Research.
[27] Dave M. Stampe,et al. Heuristic filtering and reliable calibration methods for video-based pupil-tracking systems , 1993 .
[28] Jakob Heinzle,et al. A Microcircuit Model of the Frontal Eye Fields , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[29] D. Rubin,et al. Inference from Iterative Simulation Using Multiple Sequences , 1992 .
[30] Evan M. Palmer,et al. Reaction time distributions constrain models of visual search , 2010, Vision Research.
[31] Imran Noorani,et al. LATER models of neural decision behavior in choice tasks , 2014, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[32] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Diffusion versus linear ballistic accumulation: different models but the same conclusions about psychological processes? , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[33] Karl J. Friston,et al. Comparing Families of Dynamic Causal Models , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[34] Karl J. Friston,et al. Computational neuroimaging strategies for single patient predictions , 2017, NeuroImage.
[35] Stephen Grossberg,et al. How laminar frontal cortex and basal ganglia circuits interact to control planned and reactive saccades , 2004, Neural Networks.
[36] Eduardo A. Aponte,et al. mpdcm: A toolbox for massively parallel dynamic causal modeling , 2016, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[37] Patrick W. Stroman,et al. Impaired executive function signals in motor brain regions in Parkinson's disease , 2012, NeuroImage.
[38] Patrick G. Bissett,et al. The Countermanding Task Revisited: Mimicry of Race Models , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[39] Imran Noorani,et al. Towards a unifying mechanism for cancelling movements , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[40] David J. C. MacKay,et al. Information Theory, Inference, and Learning Algorithms , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Information Theory.
[41] Daniel K. Leventhal,et al. Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathways , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.
[42] M. Frank,et al. Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] Karl J. Friston,et al. Erratum: Computational psychiatry [Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (2012), 72–80] , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] Ole E. Barndorff-Nielsen,et al. First hitting time models for the generalized inverse Gaussian distribution , 1978 .
[45] Veit Stuphorn,et al. Cortical control and performance monitoring of interrupting and redirecting movements , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[46] G. D. Logan,et al. Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque , 2007, Vision Research.
[47] R. Walker,et al. A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[48] Matthew Heath,et al. Task-switching in oculomotor control: Unidirectional switch-cost when alternating between pro- and antisaccades , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.
[49] Mingsha Zhang,et al. Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades , 2000, Nature.
[50] Vassilis Cutsuridis,et al. Behavioural and computational varieties of response inhibition in eye movements , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[51] G. Logan,et al. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. , 2014, Psychological review.
[52] Matcheri S Keshavan,et al. Longitudinal studies of antisaccades in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia , 2006, Psychological Medicine.
[53] Jeffrey D Schall,et al. Models of inhibitory control , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[54] Martin P Paulus,et al. A Roadmap for the Development of Applied Computational Psychiatry. , 2016, Biological psychiatry. Cognitive neuroscience and neuroimaging.
[55] Jordan E. Pierce,et al. Modulation of cognitive control levels via manipulation of saccade trial-type probability assessed with event-related BOLD fMRI. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[56] Godfrey D Pearlson,et al. Elevated antisaccade error rate as an intermediate phenotype for psychosis across diagnostic categories. , 2014, Schizophrenia bulletin.
[57] J. Gold,et al. The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.
[58] Karl J. Friston,et al. Computational psychiatry , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[59] Xiao-Li Meng,et al. Simulating Normalizing Constants: From Importance Sampling to Bridge Sampling to Path Sampling , 1998 .
[60] Scott D. Brown,et al. The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.
[61] P. E. Hallett,et al. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.
[62] Stavros J. Perantonis,et al. A neural model of decision-making by the superior colicullus in an antisaccade task , 2007, Neural Networks.
[63] Benedikt Reuter,et al. Poor antisaccade performance in schizophrenia: An inhibition deficit? , 2005, Psychiatry Research.
[64] Jeremy M Wolfe,et al. What are the shapes of response time distributions in visual search? , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[65] John K Kruschke,et al. Bayesian data analysis. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[66] G. Logan,et al. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[67] Michael F. Green,et al. Antisaccade performance in schizophrenia patients, their first-degree biological relatives, and community comparison subjects: data from the COGS study. , 2010, Psychophysiology.
[68] Qing Yang,et al. A Unified Comparison of Stimulus-Driven, Endogenous Mandatory and ‘Free Choice’ Saccades , 2014, PloS one.
[69] Imran Noorani,et al. Re‐starting a neural race: anti‐saccade correction , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.
[70] D. Munoz,et al. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[71] S. Hutton,et al. The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. , 2006, Psychophysiology.
[72] Vassilis Cutsuridis,et al. Antisaccade performance in schizophrenia: a neural model of decision making in the superior colliculus , 2014, Front. Neurosci..
[73] Takashi R Sato,et al. Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.
[74] Imran Noorani,et al. Full reaction time distributions reveal the complexity of neural decision‐making , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.
[75] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. Conflict Resolution as Near-Threshold Decision-Making: A Spiking Neural Circuit Model with Two-Stage Competition for Antisaccadic Task , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[76] W M Grove,et al. Saccadic disinhibition in patients with acute and remitted schizophrenia and their first-degree biological relatives. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.
[77] R. Murray,et al. Saccadic eye movements in families multiply affected with schizophrenia: the Maudsley Family Study. , 1998, The American journal of psychiatry.
[78] K. Fukushima,et al. Disturbances of voluntary control of saccadic eye movements in schizophrenic patients , 1988, Biological Psychiatry.
[79] Martin P Paulus,et al. Computational Psychiatry: From Mechanistic Insights to the Development of New Treatments. , 2016, Biological psychiatry. Cognitive neuroscience and neuroimaging.
[80] Benedikt Reuter,et al. Impaired action control in schizophrenia: The role of volitional saccade initiation , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[81] Hoon Kim,et al. Monte Carlo Statistical Methods , 2000, Technometrics.
[82] R. H. S. Carpenter,et al. Antisaccades as decisions: LATER model predicts latency distributions and error responses , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.