Neural mechanisms for forming a perceptual decision.

Cognitive and behavioral responses to environmental stimuli depend on an evaluation of sensory signals within the cerebral cortex. The mechanism by which this occurs in a specific visual task was investigated with a combination of physiological and psychophysical techniques. Rhesus monkeys discriminated among eight possible directions of motion while directional signals were manipulated in visual area MT. One directional signal was generated by a visual stimulus and a second signal was introduced by electrically stimulating neurons that encoded a specific direction of motion. The decisions made by the monkeys in response to the two signals allowed a distinction to be made between two possible mechanisms for interpreting directional signals in MT. The monkeys tended to cast decisions in favor of one or the other signal, indicating that the signals exerted independent effects on performance and that an interactive mechanism such as vector averaging of the two signals was not operative. Thus, the data suggest a mechanism in which monkeys chose the direction encoded by the largest signal in the representation of motion direction, a "winner-take-all" decision process.

[1]  D. Robinson Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. , 1972, Vision research.

[2]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[3]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[4]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.

[5]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Eye movements evoked by stimulation of frontal eye fields. , 1969, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: Myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[7]  S. Zeki Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  B. Cohen,et al.  Coding of information about rapid eye movements in the pontine reticular formation of alert monkeys , 1976, Brain Research.

[9]  V. J. Wilson,et al.  Body position with respect to the head or body position in space is coded by lumbar interneurons. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[11]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  J T McIlwain,et al.  Distributed spatial coding in the superior colliculus: A review , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[13]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  B. C. Motter,et al.  Functional properties of parietal visual neurons: radial organization of directionalities within the visual field , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects of varying pulse amplitude and frequency , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Mcilwain Lateral spread of neural excitation during microstimulation in intermediate gray layer of cat's superior colliculus. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  F. Ottes,et al.  Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: Two different modes , 1984, Vision Research.

[20]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Topographic organization of the middle temporal visual area in the macaque monkey: Representational biases and the relationship to callosal connections and myeloarchitectonic boundaries , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[23]  Hidehiko Komatsu,et al.  A grid system and a microsyringe for single cell recording , 1988, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[24]  R. Desimone,et al.  Columnar organization of directionally selective cells in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R H Schor,et al.  Responses to head tilt in cat central vestibular neurons. I. Direction of maximum sensitivity. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[27]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.

[28]  D. Sparks,et al.  Spatial localization of saccade targets. I. Compensation for stimulation-induced perturbations in eye position. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  J. Blackman,et al.  Disorder in condensed matter physics : a volume in honour of Roger Elliott , 1991 .

[30]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  V. J. Wilson,et al.  Tonic neck reflex of the decerebrate cat: response of spinal interneurons to natural stimulation of neck and vestibular receptors. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[33]  Thomas D. Albright,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual motion coherence , 1992, Nature.

[34]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction , 1990, Nature.

[35]  S. Petersen,et al.  Visual response properties of neurons in four extrastriate visual areas of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus): a quantitative comparison of medial, dorsomedial, dorsolateral, and middle temporal areas. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Roger B. H. Tootell,et al.  Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1992, Nature.

[37]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.

[38]  D. Sparks,et al.  Size and distribution of movement fields in the monkey superior colliculus , 1976, Brain Research.

[39]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Visual Motion and Its Role in the Stabilization of Gaze , 1992 .

[40]  E.,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Kenneth M. Sayre,et al.  Pattern Recognition Mechanisms and St. Thomas' Theory of Abstraction , 1963 .