Testing Problem Solving in Ravens: String-Pulling to Reach Food

The aim of our study was to re-examine the acquisition of problem-solving behaviour in ravens: accessing meat suspended from a perch by a string. In contrast to a previous study, here we: (i) controlled for possible effects of fear of the string, competition by dominants, and social learning and (ii) devised a mechanically equivalent but non-intuitive task to test for the possibility of means– end understanding. One-year-old ravens confronted with meat on a string for the first time tried several ways to reach the food. However, five of six birds suddenly performed a coherent sequence of pulling up and stepping on loops of string, essential for solving the problem. Those five birds were also successful in the nonintuitive task where they had to pull down the string to lift the meat. A second group of birds with similar exposure to strings but without any experience in pulling up meat failed the pull-down test. These results support the idea that the ravens behaviour in accessing meat on a string is not only a product of rapid learning but may involve some understanding of cause–effect relation between string, food and certain body parts.

[1]  W. Köhler The Mentality of Apes. , 2018, Nature.

[2]  Harry F. Harlow,et al.  Comparative behavior of primates. VII. Capacity of monkeys to solve patterned string tests. , 1934 .

[3]  E. Tolman The acquisition of string-pulling by rats--conditioned response or sign-gestalt? , 1937 .

[4]  R. Altevogt Über Das "Schöpfen" Einiger Vogelarten , 1954 .

[5]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[6]  M. Vince ‘String pulling’ in birds. 1. Individual differences in wild adult great tits , 1956 .

[7]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[8]  S. A. Barnett,et al.  Learning and instinct in animals. , 1957 .

[9]  M. Vince “String-pulling” in birds. (2) differences related to age in greenfinches, chaffinches and canaries , 1958 .

[10]  M. Vince "String-Pulling" in Birds. Iii. the Successful Response in Greenfinches and Canaries , 1961 .

[11]  B. Rensch,et al.  The Solution of Patterned String Problems By Birds , 1977 .

[12]  Hans Schwarz,et al.  Besprechung von: Lorenz, Konrad. Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins. Translated by M. Kerr Wilson. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1974 , 1977 .

[13]  E. Menzel Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals, Benjamin B. Beck. Garland STPM Press, New York and London (1980), 306, Price £24.50 , 1981 .

[14]  Russell P. Balda,et al.  Cache recovery and spatial memory in Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana). , 1985 .

[15]  Raven tool use , 1988 .

[16]  Tool use by the Fan-tailed Raven (Corvus Rhipidurus) , 1989 .

[17]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Foraging by common ravens in the presence and absence of territory holders: an experimental analysis of social foraging , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.

[19]  Dominance and weight changes in the common raven, Corvus corax , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  Bernd Heinrich,et al.  AN EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATION OF INSIGHT IN COMMON RAVENS (CORVUS CORAX) , 1995 .

[21]  B. Heinrich Neophilia and exploration in juvenile common ravens, Corvus corax , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  G. Hunt Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows , 1996, Nature.

[23]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Artifactual kinds and functional design features: what a primate understands without language , 1997, Cognition.

[24]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Influence of competitors on caching behaviour in the common raven,Corvus corax , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays , 1998, Nature.

[27]  K. Kotrschal,et al.  Social learning in common ravens,Corvus corax , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  Jerald D. Kralik,et al.  Problem solving and functional design features: experiments on cotton-top tamarins,Saguinus oedipus oedipus , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[29]  I. Pepperberg The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots , 2000 .

[30]  S. Tebbich,et al.  Do woodpecker finches acquire tool-use by social learning? , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  N. Emery,et al.  Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays , 2001, Nature.

[32]  K. Laland,et al.  Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Shaping of Hooks in New Caledonian Crows , 2002, Science.

[34]  Jackie Chappell,et al.  Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides) , 2002, Animal Cognition.

[35]  Bernd Heinrich,et al.  Common ravens, Corvus corax, preferentially associate with grey wolves, Canis lupus, as a foraging strategy in winter , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  T. Bugnyar,et al.  Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: is it ‘tactical’ deception? , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[37]  M. Hauser,et al.  Ontogeny of tool use in cottontop tamarins, Saguinus oedipus: innate recognition of functionally relevant features , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  Tools and brains in birds , 2002 .

[39]  Laurie R Santos,et al.  Problem solving, inhibition and domain-specific experience: experiments on cottontop tamarins, Saguinus oedipus , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  Hika Kuroshima,et al.  How do tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) understand causality involved in tool use? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[41]  Daniel J. Povinelli,et al.  The rope, hook, touching-stick, and related problems: the question of physical connection , 2003 .

[42]  Causality, tool use, and folk physics: a comparative approach , 2003 .

[43]  Jackie Chappell,et al.  Selection of tool diameter by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[44]  Lesley J. Rogers,et al.  Comparative vertebrate cognition : are primates superior to non-primates? , 2004 .

[45]  S. Tebbich,et al.  Cognitive abilities related to tool use in the woodpecker finch, Cactospiza pallida , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[46]  A. Iwaniuk,et al.  Is Cooperative Breeding Associated With Bigger Brains? A Comparative Test in the Corvida (Passeriformes) , 2004 .

[47]  N. Emery,et al.  Comparing the Complex Cognition of Birds and Primates , 2004 .

[48]  I. Pepperberg “Insightful” string-pulling in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) is affected by vocal competence , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[49]  T. Waite,et al.  Raven scavenging favours group foraging in wolves , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[50]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Do common ravens yell because they want to attract others? , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[51]  J. D. Haan Der Stieglitz als Schöpfer , 2005, Journal für Ornithologie.

[52]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding , 1989, Primates.

[53]  Bernd Heinrich,et al.  Mind of the raven : investigations and adventures with wolf-birds , 2006 .

[54]  Ludwig Huber,et al.  A case of quick problem solving in birds: string pulling in keas, Nestor notabilis , 2006, Animal Behaviour.