Measuring the Attentional Effect of the Bottom-Up Saliency Map of Natural Images

A saliency map is the bottom-up contribution to the deployment of exogenous attention. It, as well as its underlying neural mechanism, is hard to identify because of the existence of top-down signals. In order to exclude the contamination of top-down signals, invisible natural images were used as our stimuli to guide attention. The saliency map of natural images was calculated according to the model developed by Itti $et\ al.$ [1]. We found a salient region in natural images could attract attention to improve subjects' orientation discrimination performance at the salient region. Furthermore, the attraction of attention increased with the degree of saliency. Our findings suggest that the bottom-up saliency map of a natural image could be generated at a very early stage of visual processing.

[1]  J. Gallant,et al.  Goal-Related Activity in V4 during Free Viewing Visual Search Evidence for a Ventral Stream Visual Salience Map , 2003, Neuron.

[2]  F. Fang,et al.  Cortical responses to invisible objects in human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2010 .

[3]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  The footprints of visual attention in the Posner cueing paradigm revealed by classification images. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[4]  George R. Mangun,et al.  Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus is Sensitive to Bottom–Up Attention Driven by Stimulus Salience , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Fang Fang,et al.  A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  Neural Activities in V1 Create a Bottom-Up Saliency Map , 2012, Neuron.

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  Christof Koch,et al.  A Model of Saliency-Based Visual Attention for Rapid Scene Analysis , 2009 .

[9]  Z Li,et al.  Contextual influences in V1 as a basis for pop out and asymmetry in visual search. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  Karen K. De Valois,et al.  Measuring the role of chromatic saturation and luminance contrast in color spreading using hue cancellation , 2002 .

[11]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Faculty Opinions recommendation of Goal-related activity in V4 during free viewing visual search. Evidence for a ventral stream visual salience map. , 2004 .

[12]  Wen Gao,et al.  Measuring visual saliency by Site Entropy Rate , 2010, 2010 IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.

[13]  Zhaoping Li A saliency map in primary visual cortex , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  K. Bühler,et al.  Visualization of vortex breakdown structures , 1999 .

[15]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[16]  J. Haynes,et al.  Decoding Successive Computational Stages of Saliency Processing , 2011, Current Biology.

[17]  J. Allman,et al.  Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  Liqing Zhang,et al.  Saliency Detection: A Spectral Residual Approach , 2007, 2007 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.