A model using MT-like motion-opponent operators explains an illusory transformation in the optic flow field
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Wurtz,et al. Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[2] J. Perrone,et al. A model of self-motion estimation within primate extrastriate visual cortex , 1994, Vision Research.
[3] Constance S. Royden,et al. Computing heading in the presence of moving objects: a model that uses motion-opponent operators , 2002, Vision Research.
[4] R A Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. III. Modeling , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[5] G. Orban,et al. Shape and Spatial Distribution of Receptive Fields and Antagonistic Motion Surrounds in the Middle Temporal Area (V5) of the Macaque , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.
[6] A. V. van den Berg,et al. Heading detection using motion templates and eye velocity gain fields , 1998, Vision Research.
[7] Constance S. Royden,et al. Human heading judgments in the presence of moving objects , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[8] Ellen C. Hildreth,et al. Recovering heading for visually-guided navigation , 1992, Vision Research.
[9] M. Graziano,et al. Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[10] T J Sejnowski,et al. A Model for Encoding Multiple Object Motions and Self-Motion in Area MST of Primate Visual Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[11] W H Warren,et al. Perceiving Heading in the Presence of Moving Objects , 1995, Perception.
[12] G. Orban,et al. Spatial heterogeneity of inhibitory surrounds in the middle temporal visual area. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[13] Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos,et al. Visual navigation with a neural network , 1991, Neural Networks.
[14] Constance S. Royden,et al. Mathematical analysis of motion-opponent mechanisms used in the determination of heading and depth. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[15] R A Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. I. Psychophysics , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[16] J Allman,et al. Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.
[17] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[18] M. Banks,et al. Perceiving heading with different retinal regions and types of optic flow , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[19] R. Wurtz,et al. An illusory transformation of optic flow fields , 1993, Vision Research.
[20] Daniel J. Hannon,et al. Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.
[21] D C Van Essen,et al. Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] T. Meese,et al. Induced motion may account for the illusory transformation of optic flow fields found by Duffy and Wurtz , 1995, Vision Research.
[23] J. H. Rieger,et al. Human visual navigation in the presence of 3-D rotations , 1985, Biological Cybernetics.
[24] Markus Lappe,et al. A Neural Network for the Processing of Optic Flow from Ego-Motion in Man and Higher Mammals , 1993, Neural Computation.
[25] M. Lappe,et al. Interaction of stereo vision and optic flow processing revealed by an illusory stimulus , 1998, Vision Research.
[26] D J Hannon,et al. Eye movements and optical flow. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[27] H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al. The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[28] C. Duffy,et al. Optic flow illusion and single neuron behaviour reconciled by a population model , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.
[29] J A Perrone,et al. Model for the computation of self-motion in biological systems. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[30] R. Wurtz,et al. Response of monkey MST neurons to optic flow stimuli with shifted centers of motion , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[31] J H Rieger,et al. Processing differential image motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[32] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[33] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] A. V. D. Berg,et al. Robustness of perception of heading from optic flow , 1992, Vision Research.
[35] Paul A. Braren,et al. Wayfinding on foot from information in retinal, not optical, flow. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[36] Markus Lappe,et al. An illusory transformation in a model of optic flow processing , 1995, Vision Research.
[37] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[38] R. Wurtz,et al. Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. II. Mechanisms of response selectivity revealed by small-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[39] Ennio Mingolla,et al. Global induced motion and visual stability in an optic flow illusion , 1997, Vision Research.
[40] J. Gibson. The perception of the visual world , 1951 .