Adaptive explanations for sensitive windows in development

Development in many organisms appears to show evidence of sensitive windows—periods or stages in ontogeny in which individual experience has a particularly strong influence on the phenotype (compared to other periods or stages). Despite great interest in sensitive windows from both fundamental and applied perspectives, the functional (adaptive) reasons why they have evolved are unclear. Here we outline a conceptual framework for understanding when natural selection should favour changes in plasticity across development. Our approach builds on previous theory on the evolution of phenotypic plasticity, which relates individual and population differences in plasticity to two factors: the degree of uncertainty about the environmental conditions and the extent to which experiences during development (‘cues’) provide information about those conditions. We argue that systematic variation in these two factors often occurs within the lifetime of a single individual, which will select for developmental changes in plasticity. Of central importance is how informational properties of the environment interact with the life history of the organism. Phenotypes may be more or less sensitive to environmental cues at different points in development because of systematic changes in (i) the frequency of cues, (ii) the informativeness of cues, (iii) the fitness benefits of information and/or (iv) the constraints on plasticity. In relatively stable environments, a sensible null expectation is that plasticity will gradually decline with age as the developing individual gathers information. We review recent models on the evolution of developmental changes in plasticity and explain how they fit into our conceptual framework. Our aim is to encourage an adaptive perspective on sensitive windows in development.

[1]  P. Medawar UNSOLVED problem of biology. , 1953, The Medical journal of Australia.

[2]  W. Hamilton The moulding of senescence by natural selection. , 1966, Journal of theoretical biology.

[3]  Klaus Immelmann,et al.  Sexual and Other Long-Term Aspects of Imprinting in Birds and Other Species , 1972 .

[4]  R. Fernald,et al.  Field study of Haplochromis burtoni: Quantitative behavioural observations , 1977, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  P. Bateson How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  R. Law Optimal Life Histories Under Age-Specific Predation , 1979, The American Naturalist.

[7]  Kevin MacDonald,et al.  Early experience, relative plasticity, and social development , 1985 .

[8]  J. Riley,et al.  The analytics of uncertainty and information: Long-run relationships and the credibility of threats and promises , 1992 .

[9]  Scott P Carroll,et al.  Divergence in male mating tactics between two populations of the soapberry bug: II. Genetic change and the evolution of a plastic reaction norm in a variable social environment , 1995 .

[10]  A. Lucas,et al.  Programming by early nutrition: an experimental approach. , 1998, The Journal of nutrition.

[11]  D. Wilson,et al.  Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity. , 1998, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[12]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Capacity for plasticity in the adult owl auditory system expanded by juvenile experience. , 1998, Science.

[13]  J. Lindström,et al.  Early development and fitness in birds and mammals. , 1999, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[14]  C. ten Cate,et al.  Sexual imprinting and evolutionary processes in birds: A reassessment , 1999 .

[15]  R. Fernald,et al.  Social status regulates growth rate: consequences for life-history strategies. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  L. Spear The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  B. Charlesworth,et al.  Fisher, Medawar, Hamilton and the evolution of aging. , 2000, Genetics.

[18]  M A Nowak,et al.  Natural selection of the critical period for language acquisition , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  S. White,et al.  Social regulation of gonadotropin-releasing hormone. , 2002, The Journal of experimental biology.

[20]  Jean Clobert,et al.  Hormones, developmental plasticity and adaptation , 2002 .

[21]  R. Relyea Costs of Phenotypic Plasticity , 2002, The American Naturalist.

[22]  Frederic Mery,et al.  A fitness cost of learning ability in Drosophila melanogaster , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Edward F Chang,et al.  Environmental Noise Retards Auditory Cortical Development , 2003, Science.

[24]  D. Gesellschaft Frontiers in zoology , 2004 .

[25]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective , 2004 .

[26]  Russell D. Fernald,et al.  Field study of Haplochromis burtoni: habitats and co-habitant , 1977, Environmental Biology of Fishes.

[27]  P. Gowaty Developmental Plasticity and Evolution Mary Jane West-Eberhard , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  C. Kuzawa Fetal origins of developmental plasticity: Are fetal cues reliable predictors of future nutritional environments? , 2005, American journal of human biology : the official journal of the Human Biology Council.

[29]  Frederic Mery,et al.  A Cost of Long-Term Memory in Drosophila , 2005, Science.

[30]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[31]  K. Lorenz,et al.  Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels , 1935, Journal für Ornithologie.

[32]  K. Lorenz,et al.  Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels , 1935, Journal für Ornithologie.

[33]  B. Taborsky Mothers determine offspring size in response to own juvenile growth conditions , 2006, Biology Letters.

[34]  Tobias Bonhoeffer,et al.  Prior experience enhances plasticity in adult visual cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  Ola Olsson,et al.  Bayes' theorem and its applications in animal behaviour , 2006 .

[36]  David W. Stephens,et al.  What makes information valuable: signal reliability and environmental uncertainty , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[37]  T. Uller,et al.  When is a maternal effect adaptive , 2007 .

[38]  R. Relyea,et al.  How flexible is phenotypic plasticity? Developmental windows for trait induction and reversal. , 2007, Ecology.

[39]  Franz J. Weissing,et al.  Evolutionary emergence of responsive and unresponsive personalities , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[40]  Alexander Lange,et al.  Bayesian Approximations and Extensions: Optimal Decisions for Small Brains and Possibly Big Ones Too , 2022 .

[41]  A. Agrawal,et al.  Re-evaluating the costs and limits of adaptive phenotypic plasticity , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  T. W. Fawcett,et al.  Previous experiences shape adaptive mate preferences , 2009 .

[43]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[44]  T. Laaksonen,et al.  Yolk hormones have sex-specific long-term effects on behavior in the pied flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca) , 2010, Hormones and Behavior.

[45]  Carl T. Bergstrom,et al.  The fitness value of information , 2005, Oikos.

[46]  T. W. Fawcett,et al.  change with age and experience Learning your own strength : winner and loser effects should , 2010 .

[47]  Judy Stamps,et al.  The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives , 2010, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[48]  B. Taborsky,et al.  Environmental Change Enhances Cognitive Abilities in Fish , 2010, PLoS biology.

[49]  B. Ellis,et al.  The mating sociometer: a regulatory mechanism for mating aspirations. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  R. McKay,et al.  The subtleties of error management , 2010 .

[51]  John M. McNamara,et al.  Information is a fitness enhancing resource , 2010 .

[52]  Willem E. Frankenhuis,et al.  Individual Differences in Developmental Plasticity May Result From Stochastic Sampling , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[53]  W. Frankenhuis,et al.  Balancing sampling and specialization: an adaptationist model of incremental development , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[54]  James A. R. Marshall,et al.  Decision-making under uncertainty: biases and Bayesians , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[55]  R. Valentino,et al.  Early Adolescence as a Critical Window During Which Social Stress Distinctly Alters Behavior and Brain Norepinephrine Activity , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[56]  J. Koolhaas,et al.  Social behavior and social stress in adolescence: A focus on animal models , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[57]  Jay Belsky,et al.  Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory , 2011, Development and Psychopathology.

[58]  R. Fernald,et al.  Social information changes the brain , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  M. Del Giudice,et al.  The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: implications for science, policy, and practice. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[60]  R. Holt,et al.  The genetics of phenotypic plasticity. X. Variation versus uncertainty , 2012, Ecology and evolution.

[61]  C. DeWall,et al.  The optimal calibration hypothesis: how life history modulates the brain's social pain network , 2012, Front. Evol. Neurosci..

[62]  Kimberley J. Mathot,et al.  Adaptive strategies for managing uncertainty may explain personality-related differences in behavioural plasticity , 2012 .

[63]  Adolescent social environment shapes sexual and aggressive behaviour of adult male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) , 2013, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[64]  Fetal programming by maternal stress: Insights from a conflict perspective. , 2012, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[65]  M. Giudice Fetal programming by maternal stress: Insights from a conflict perspective , 2012, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[66]  E. Snell-Rood,et al.  An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural plasticity , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[67]  P. O’Donnell,et al.  Early Social Experience Is Critical for the Development of Cognitive Control and Dopamine Modulation of Prefrontal Cortex Function , 2013, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[68]  Barbara Fischer,et al.  The Evolution of Age-Dependent Plasticity , 2013, The American Naturalist.

[69]  Alasdair I Houston,et al.  On evolutionary explanations of cognitive biases. , 2013, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[70]  H. Bischof,et al.  Social experience during adolescence influences how male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) group with conspecifics , 2013, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[71]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Exposing the behavioral gambit: the evolution of learning and decision rules , 2013 .

[72]  R. Fernald,et al.  Social regulation of male reproductive plasticity in an African cichlid fish. , 2013, Integrative and comparative biology.

[73]  W. Frankenhuis,et al.  Bridging developmental systems theory and evolutionary psychology using dynamic optimization. , 2013, Developmental science.

[74]  Animal Personalities: Behavior, Physiology, and Evolution , 2013 .

[75]  F. Champagne,et al.  Early interactions with mother and peers independently build adult social skills and shape BDNF and oxytocin receptor brain levels , 2013, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[76]  W. Frankenhuis,et al.  The evolution of predictive adaptive responses in human life history , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[77]  T. Uller,et al.  Weak evidence for anticipatory parental effects in plants and animals , 2013, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[78]  N. Sachser,et al.  Behavioural profiles are shaped by social experience: when, how and why , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[79]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  The evolution of decision rules in complex environments , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[80]  Oliver M. Lean Getting the most out of Shannon information , 2014 .

[81]  F. Weissing,et al.  Evolutionary tipping points in the capacity to adapt to environmental change , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[82]  V. V. Krishnan,et al.  Combining Information from Ancestors and Personal Experiences to Predict Individual Differences in Developmental Trajectories , 2014, The American Naturalist.

[83]  M. Del Giudice Early stress and human behavioral development: emerging evolutionary perspectives , 2014, Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease.

[84]  M. D. Giudice,et al.  Early stress and human behavioral development: emerging evolutionary perspectives. , 2014 .

[85]  Marco Del Giudice,et al.  Middle Childhood: An Evolutionary‐Developmental Synthesis , 2014 .

[86]  J. Wells Implications for the programming of obesity , 2014 .

[87]  V. V. Krishnan,et al.  Individual differences in the potential and realized developmental plasticity of personality traits , 2014, Front. Ecol. Evol..

[88]  Scott C. Burgess,et al.  Adaptive parental effects: the importance of estimating environmental predictability and offspring fitness appropriately , 2014 .

[89]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? , 2014, Annual review of psychology.

[90]  B. Taborsky,et al.  Antipredator defences of young are independently determined by genetic inheritance, maternal effects and own early experience in mouthbrooding cichlids. , 2014 .

[91]  N. Sachser,et al.  Stability and change: Stress responses and the shaping of behavioral phenotypes over the life span , 2015, Frontiers in Zoology.

[92]  R. Gomulkiewicz,et al.  The Genetics of Phenotypic Plasticity , 2015 .

[93]  N. Raihani,et al.  Developmental plasticity and social specialization in cooperative societies , 2015, Animal Behaviour.

[94]  J. Brommer,et al.  The importance of genotype-by-age interactions for the development of repeatable behavior and correlated behaviors over lifetime , 2015, Frontiers in Zoology.

[95]  M. Del Giudice Plasticity as a developing trait: exploring the implications , 2015, Frontiers in Zoology.

[96]  L. Lewejohann,et al.  Lifetime development of behavioural phenotype in the house mouse (Mus musculus) , 2015, Frontiers in Zoology.

[97]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Brain adaptation and alternative developmental trajectories , 2015, Development and Psychopathology.

[98]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  Adaptive Use of Information during Growth Can Explain Long-Term Effects of Early Life Experiences , 2016, The American Naturalist.

[99]  W. Frankenhuis,et al.  The evolution of sensitive periods in a model of incremental development , 2016, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[100]  J. Belsky,et al.  A mathematical model of the evolution of individual differences in developmental plasticity arising through parental bet-hedging. , 2016, Developmental science.