Metacognitive knowledge about problem-solving methods.

BACKGROUND The effective application of a problem-solving method requires the knowledge of what task is relevant, what the abilities involved are and how much effort is needed. However, as yet too little is known about these metacognitive representations. AIM This study was aimed at describing beliefs about problem-solving methods and at assessing whether they vary according to the kind of method and of problem and are modified by psychological courses attended. SAMPLE Forty-six Italian undergraduates in psychology and 37 in non-psychological disciplines. METHODS Participants had to rate how frequently each of five problem-solving methods (free production, analogy, step-by-step analysis, visualisation and combining) is employed and how effective and easy each one is to apply. Ratings were requested for interpersonal, practical and study problems. Participants were also asked to identify which abilities they thought would be involved in each method. RESULTS According to students' ratings, the most frequently used problem-solving method was analogy, which was also considered the easiest method to apply, whereas step-by-step analysis and combining were considered the most difficult. Problem-solving techniques were perceived as being relevant above all for practical problems, whereas they were conceived as less suitable for interpersonal problems. For study problems the most relevant strategy was step-by-step analysis. Students were aware of the abilities relevant to each problem-solving method. CONCLUSIONS Undergraduates both in psychology and non-psychological disciplines can identify some critical features in the methods used to solve problems, even though some misconceptions emerged. Since metacognition plays a causal role in problem-solving, trainers should take into account trainees' folk representations of problem-solving strategies.

[1]  W. Gordon Synectics: The Development of Creative Capacity , 1961 .

[2]  S. Mednick The associative basis of the creative process. , 1962, Psychological review.

[3]  F. Zwicky Discovery, Invention, Research through the morphological approach , 1969 .

[4]  N. A. Rahman,et al.  How to solve problems : elements of a theory of problems and problem solving , 1975 .

[5]  The Complete Thinker: A Handbook of Techniques for Creative and Critical Problem Solving , 1980 .

[6]  Kenneth R. Pfeiffer,et al.  Concepts in problem solving , 1980 .

[7]  John R. Hayes,et al.  The Complete Problem Solver , 1981 .

[8]  Dean Keith Simonton,et al.  Genius, creativity, and leadership , 1984 .

[9]  Alan H. Schoenfeld,et al.  Mathematical Problem Solving , 1985 .

[10]  B. Slife,et al.  Separability of Metacognition and Cognition: Problem Solving in Learning Disabled and Regular Students. , 1985 .

[11]  R. Sternberg,et al.  Intelligence Applied: Understanding and Increasing Your Intellectual Skills , 1986 .

[12]  A. Ortony,et al.  Similarity and Analogical Reasoning , 1991 .

[13]  Bonnie B. Armbruster Metacognition in Creativity , 1989 .

[14]  H. Swanson Influence of Metacognitive Knowledge and Aptitude on Problem Solving. , 1990 .

[15]  Alessandro Antonietti,et al.  Chapter 15 Why does mental visualization facilitate problem-solving? , 1991 .

[16]  P. Johnson-Laird Human and Machine Thinking , 1992 .

[17]  H. Swanson The Relationship between Metacognition and Problem Solving in Gifted Children. , 1992 .

[18]  Eleanor Armour-Thomas,et al.  Construct Validation of Metacognition , 1993 .

[19]  R. Weisberg Creativity: Beyond the Myth of Genius , 1993 .

[20]  Edward Lumsdaine,et al.  Creative problem solving , 1995 .

[21]  R. Sternberg Handbook of Creativity: Subject Index , 1998 .