The influence of prior record on moral judgment

Repeat offenders are commonly given more severe sentences than first-time offenders for the same violations. Though this practice makes intuitive sense, the theory behind escalating penalties is disputed in both legal and economic theories. Here we investigate folk intuitions concerning the moral and intentional status of actions performed by people with positive versus negative prior records. We hypothesized that prior record would modulate both moral judgment and mental state reasoning. Subjects first engaged in an economic game with fair (positive prior record) and unfair (negative prior record) competitors and then read descriptions of their competitors' actions that resulted in either positive or negative outcomes. The descriptions left the competitors' mental states unstated. We found that subjects judged actions producing negative outcomes as more "intentional" and more "blameworthy" when performed by unfair competitors. Although explicit mental state evaluation was not required, moral judgments in this case were accompanied by increased activation in brain regions associated with mental state reasoning, including predominantly the right temporo-parietal junction (RTPJ). The magnitude of RTPJ activation was correlated with individual subjects' behavioural responses to unfair play in the game. These results thus provide insight for both legal theory and moral psychology.

[1]  Thomas Nadelhoffer On Praise, Side Effects, and Folk Ascriptions of Intentionality. , 2004 .

[2]  Barbara A Mellers,et al.  Emotions and cooperation in economic games. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  D. Perrett,et al.  A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  T. Mexia,et al.  Author ' s personal copy , 2009 .

[5]  Peter Salovey,et al.  Asymmetry in Judgments of Moral Blame and Praise , 2003, Psychological science.

[6]  E. Loftus,et al.  Ripple effects in memory: judgments of moral blame can distort memory for events , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[7]  R. Saxe,et al.  The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  John M. Frost,et al.  Moral heuristics. Commentaries. Author's reply , 2005 .

[9]  F. Solina 15 seconds of fame , 2004, Leonardo.

[10]  J. Haidt The New Synthesis in Moral Psychology , 2007, Science.

[11]  J. Tooby Intention, intentional action and moral considerations , 2004 .

[12]  J. Knobe Intentional Action and Side Effects in Ordinary Language , 2003 .

[13]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain , 2004, Science.

[14]  David A. Dana Rethinking the Puzzle of Escalating Penalties for Repeat Offenders , 2001 .

[15]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[16]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  Theory of mind and moral cognition: exploring the connections , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[18]  J. Darley,et al.  Identification, situational constraint, and social cognition: Studies in the attribution of moral responsibility , 2006, Cognition.

[19]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[20]  C. Sunstein,et al.  moral heuristics , 2003, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment , 2008, NeuroImage.

[22]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[23]  John Mikhail Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.4 Universal moral grammar: theory, evidence and the future , 2022 .

[24]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[25]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  The Pervasive Impact of Moral Judgment , 2009 .

[26]  A. Ashworth Sentencing and criminal justice , 2005 .

[27]  Josef Perner,et al.  Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? , 2006, NeuroImage.

[28]  D. Tranel,et al.  Irrational Economic Decision-Making after Ventromedial Prefrontal Damage: Evidence from the Ultimatum Game , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[30]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others , 2006, Nature.

[31]  M. Hauser,et al.  The Role of Conscious Reasoning and Intuition in Moral Judgment , 2006, Psychological science.

[32]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[33]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Spontaneous Mental State Inference for Moral Judgment , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[36]  Joyce E. Berg,et al.  Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History , 1995 .

[37]  M. Rabin Published by: American , 2022 .

[38]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[39]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Aaron C. Koralek,et al.  Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  H. Walter,et al.  The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of intentions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Franc Solina,et al.  COLOR-BASED FACE DETECTION IN THE "15 SECONDS OF FAME" ART INSTALLATION , 2003 .

[43]  Mark Phelan,et al.  The folk strike back; or, why you didn’t do it intentionally, though it was bad and you knew it , 2008 .

[44]  Ori Friedman,et al.  Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’ , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  The folk concepts of intention and intentional action: A cross-cultural study , 2006 .

[46]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[47]  J. Flavell,et al.  Cognitive development: children's knowledge about the mind. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[48]  R. Dolan,et al.  Brain Responses to the Acquired Moral Status of Faces , 2004, Neuron.

[49]  Thomas Nadelhoffer Blame, Badness, and Intentional Action: A Reply to Knobe and Mendlow. , 2004 .

[50]  R. Saxe Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[51]  D. D. de Quervain,et al.  The Neural Basis of Altruistic Punishment , 2004, Science.

[52]  Winand Emons Escalating Penalties for Repeat Offenders , 2003 .

[53]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements , 2007, Nature.

[55]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[56]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  The Concept of Intentional Action: A Case Study in the Uses of Folk Psychology , 2006 .

[57]  J. Decety,et al.  What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective‐taking , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[58]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  Acting Intentionally and the Side-Effect Effect , 2006, Psychological science.