Reasoning and dyslexia: a spatial strategy may impede reasoning with visually rich information.

Bacon, Handley, and Newstead (2003, 2004), have presented evidence for individual differences in reasoning strategies, with most people seeming to represent and manipulate problem information using either a verbal or a spatial strategy. There is also evidence that individuals with dyslexia are inclined to conceptualise information in a visuo-spatial, rather than a verbal, way (e.g. von Károlyi et al., 2003). If so, we might expect a higher proportion of individuals with dyslexia to be spatial reasoners, compared with individuals who do not have dyslexia. The study reported here directly compared strategies reported by these two groups of participants on a syllogistic reasoning task. Moreover, problem content was manipulated so that reasoning across concrete and abstract materials could be compared. The findings suggest that whilst most individuals without dyslexia use a verbal strategy, reasoners with dyslexia do tend to adopt a spatial approach, though their performance is impaired with visually concrete materials. However, when reasoning with more abstract content, they perform comparably with non-dyslexic controls. The paper discusses these results in the light of recent research which has suggested that visual images may impede reasoning, and considers how individuals with dyslexia may differ from other reasoners.

[1]  S. Pickering,et al.  Development of Memory for Pattern and Path: Further Evidence for the Fractionation of Visuo-Spatial Memory , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[2]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  Human Reasoning: The Psychology Of Deduction , 1993 .

[3]  E L Grigorenko,et al.  Developmental dyslexia: an update on genes, brains, and environments. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[4]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[5]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Visual imagery can impede reasoning , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[6]  Gerhard Strube,et al.  Spatial Reasoning: No Need for Visual Information , 2001, COSIT.

[7]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[8]  Ellen Winner,et al.  Dyslexia and Visual-Spatial Talents: Compensation vs Deficit Model , 2001, Brain and Language.

[9]  Cynthia A. Riccio,et al.  Neuroanatomical and neurophysiological aspects of dyslexia , 1996 .

[10]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human prefrontal cortex activation during a spatial working memory task. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  The Neural Bases of Strategy and Skill in Sentence–Picture Verification , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  D. Kimura,et al.  The asymmetry of the human brain. , 1973, Scientific American.

[13]  Stephen E. Newstead,et al.  Individual differences in strategies for syllogistic reasoning , 2003 .

[14]  John Everatt,et al.  An eye for the unusual: creative thinking in dyslexics , 1999 .

[15]  A. Garnham,et al.  Thinking and Reasoning , 1994 .

[16]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Phonological Processes, Confrontational Naming, and Immediate Memory in Dyslexia , 1993, Journal of learning disabilities.

[17]  Gerald C. Cupchik,et al.  Emerging visions of the aesthetic process : psychology, semiology, and philosophy , 1995 .

[18]  S. Pickering,et al.  Cognitive Approaches to the Fractionation of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory , 2001, Cortex.

[19]  Marilyn Ford,et al.  Two modes of mental representation and problem solution in syllogistic reasoning , 1995, Cognition.

[20]  Angela J. Fawcett Dyslexia : theory and good practice , 2001 .

[21]  Thomas G. West,et al.  In the Mind's Eye: Visual Thinkers, Gifted People with Dyslexia and Other Learning Difficulties, Computer Images and the Ironies of Creativity , 1997 .

[22]  Markus Knauff,et al.  Mental imagery, reasoning, and blindness , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Facial recognition and brain asymmetries: Clues to underlying mechanisms , 1983, Annals of neurology.

[24]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuo–spatial memory , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Roderick I. Nicolson,et al.  Investigating the central executive in adult dyslexics: Evidence from phonological and visuospatial working memory performance , 2003 .

[26]  P. Warr,et al.  Copyright © The British Psychological Society Unauthorised use and reproduction in any form (including the internet and other electronic means) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society. , 2005 .

[27]  A. Fawcett,et al.  Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research? , 1990, Cognition.

[28]  A. Galaburda The Dyslexic Brain. (Book Reviews: Dyslexia and Development. Neurobiological Aspects of Extra-Ordinary Brains.) , 1993 .

[29]  Maxwell J. Roberts,et al.  Methods of thought : individual differences in reasoning strategies , 2004 .

[30]  David McLoughlin,et al.  Adult Dyslexia: Assessment, Counselling and Training , 1994 .

[31]  Jon Oberlander,et al.  A cognitive theory of graphical and linguistic reasoning: logic and implementation. Cognitive Science , 1995 .

[32]  H. Cohen,et al.  Oral Language Deficits in Dyslexic Children: Weaknesses in Working Memory and Verbal Planning , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[33]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental imagery and dyslexia: A deficit in processing multipart visual objects? , 1991, Brain and Language.

[34]  Ellen Winner,et al.  Dyslexia linked to talent: Global visual-spatial ability , 2003, Brain and Language.