Contextual effects on the perceived health benefits of exercise: the exercise rank hypothesis.

Many accounts of social influences on exercise participation describe how people compare their behaviors to those of others. We develop and test a novel hypothesis, the exercise rank hypothesis, of how this comparison can occur. The exercise rank hypothesis, derived from evolutionary theory and the decision by sampling model of judgment, suggests that individuals' perceptions of the health benefits of exercise are influenced by how individuals believe the amount of exercise ranks in comparison with other people's amounts of exercise. Study 1 demonstrated that individuals' perceptions of the health benefits of their own current exercise amounts were as predicted by the exercise rank hypothesis. Study 2 demonstrated that the perceptions of the health benefits of an amount of exercise can be manipulated by experimentally changing the ranked position of the amount within a comparison context. The discussion focuses on how social norm-based interventions could benefit from using rank information.

[1]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[2]  L. Cronbach Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests , 1951 .

[3]  T. Newton,et al.  Dominance, gender, and cardiovascular reactivity during social interaction. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[4]  D. Langemo,et al.  The relationship of age, gender, and exercise practices to measures of health, life-style, and self-esteem. , 1990, Applied nursing research : ANR.

[5]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Decision by sampling , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  A. Wood,et al.  Social norm influences on evaluations of the risks associated with alcohol consumption: applying the rank-based decision by sampling model to health judgments. , 2012, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[7]  I. Mysterud,et al.  Evolutionary health promotion. , 2002, Preventive medicine.

[8]  A. Wood,et al.  Am I Abnormal? Relative Rank and Social Norm Effects in Judgments of Anxiety and Depression Symptom Severity , 2013 .

[9]  C. Bouchard,et al.  Physical Activity and Health , 2006 .

[10]  Albert V. Carron,et al.  Social influence and exercise: A meta-analysis. , 1996 .

[11]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Organic Fitness: Physical Activity Consistent with our Hunter-Gatherer Heritage , 2010, The Physician and sportsmedicine.

[12]  K. Courneya,et al.  Identifying Belief-Based Targets for the Promotion of Leisure-Time Walking , 2009, Health education & behavior : the official publication of the Society for Public Health Education.

[13]  Robert Vogel,et al.  Exercise like a hunter-gatherer: a prescription for organic physical fitness. , 2011, Progress in cardiovascular diseases.

[14]  Keith. Kendall Nudge – Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth and Happiness , 2010 .

[15]  K. Spink,et al.  When in Rome: Descriptive norms and physical activity , 2011 .

[16]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Cognitive antecedents to adolescent health risk: Discriminating between behavioral intention and behavioral willingness , 1998 .

[17]  P. Dolan,et al.  Influencing behaviour: The mindspace way , 2012 .

[18]  R. Thaler,et al.  Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness , 2008 .

[19]  R. Sapolsky Social Status and Health in Humans and Other Animals , 2004 .

[20]  Amalia Waxman,et al.  Who Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity and Health * , 2004, Food and nutrition bulletin.

[21]  Paul Kline,et al.  A Handbook of Test Construction : Introduction to Psychometric Design , 1987 .

[22]  N. Sanders,et al.  Journal of behavioral decision making: "The need for contextual and technical knowledge in judgmental forecasting", 5 (1992) 39-52 , 1992 .

[23]  Carl A. Kallgren,et al.  A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: A Theoretical Refinement and Reevaluation of the Role of Norms in Human Behavior , 1991 .

[24]  Gösta Samuelson,et al.  Global strategy on diet, physical activity and health , 2004 .

[25]  S. Messick Validity of Psychological Assessment: Validation of Inferences from Persons' Responses and Performances as Scientific Inquiry into Score Meaning. Research Report RR-94-45. , 1994 .

[26]  Allen Parducci,et al.  Category rating scales: Effects of relative spacing and frequency , 1971 .

[27]  Ulf Ekelund,et al.  Assessment of physical activity in youth. , 2008, Journal of applied physiology.

[28]  G. Holton Sociobiology: the new synthesis? , 1977, Newsletter on science, technology & human values.

[29]  Edward L. Deci,et al.  Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior , 1975, Perspectives in Social Psychology.

[30]  C. Currie,et al.  Young people's health in context. Health Behaviour in School-aged Childre (HBSC) study: international report from the 2001/2002 study. , 2004 .

[31]  Denise Dellarosa Cummins,et al.  Dominance hierarchies and the evolution of human reasoning , 1996, Minds and Machines.

[32]  P. Gooding,et al.  The role of defeat and entrapment in depression, anxiety, and suicide. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  S. Messick Validity of Psychological Assessment: Validation of Inferences from Persons' Responses and Performances as Scientific Inquiry into Score Meaning. Research Report RR-94-45. , 1994 .

[34]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Using Images to Increase Exercise Behavior: Prototypes Versus Possible Selves , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[35]  W. Velicer,et al.  Comparison of five rules for determining the number of components to retain. , 1986 .

[36]  Allen Parducci,et al.  Effects of context in judgments of sweetness and pleasantness , 1979 .

[37]  M. Pratt,et al.  Physical activity and health , 1996, BMJ.

[38]  John Maltby,et al.  How are personality judgments made? A cognitive model of reference group effects, personality scale responses, and behavioral reactions. , 2012, Journal of personality.