Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children

Five experiments investigated whether adults and preschool children can perform simple arithmetic calculations on non-symbolic numerosities. Previous research has demonstrated that human adults, human infants, and non-human animals can process numerical quantities through approximate representations of their magnitudes. Here we consider whether these non-symbolic numerical representations might serve as a building block of uniquely human, learned mathematics. Both adults and children with no training in arithmetic successfully performed approximate arithmetic on large sets of elements. Success at these tasks did not depend on non-numerical continuous quantities, modality-specific quantity information, the adoption of alternative non-arithmetic strategies, or learned symbolic arithmetic knowledge. Abstract numerical quantity representations therefore are computationally functional and may provide a foundation for formal mathematics.

[1]  P. MacNeilage,et al.  Numerical representations in primates. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  永福 智志 The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2007, Developmental science.

[4]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Amount Versus Number: Infants' Use of Area and Contour Length to Discriminate Small Sets , 2001 .

[5]  Susan Carey,et al.  Evidence for numerical abilities in young infants: a fatal flaw? , 2002 .

[6]  David C. Geary,et al.  Reflections of evolution and culture in children's cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. , 1995 .

[7]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  The evolution and ontogeny of ordinal numerical ability , 2002 .

[9]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[10]  T. Simon,et al.  Reconceptualizing the Origins of Number Knowledge: A "Non-Numerical" Account , 1997 .

[11]  Negative priming from activation of counting and addition knowledge , 2001, Psychological research.

[12]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[13]  Susan C. Levine,et al.  Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and low-income families. , 1992 .

[14]  E. Spelke,et al.  Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.

[15]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[16]  S. Dehaene Subtracting Pigeons: Logarithmic or Linear? , 2001, Psychological science.

[17]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.

[18]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Self-stimulating rats combine subjective reward magnitude and subjective reward rate multiplicatively. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  D. Geary,et al.  Reflections of evolution and culture in children's cognition. Implications for mathematical development and instruction. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[20]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  First Principles Organize Attention to and Learning About Relevant Data: Number and the Animate-Inanimate Distinction as Examples , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[21]  R. Baayen,et al.  Affixal Homonymy triggers full-form storage, even with inflected words, even in a morphologically rich language , 2000, Cognition.

[22]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[23]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[24]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[26]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton–top tamarins , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  E. Spelke,et al.  The construction of large number representations in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[28]  R. Church,et al.  Alternative representations of time, number, and rate , 1990, Cognition.

[29]  Susan Carey,et al.  Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: Examinations of content and format , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  M. Hauser,et al.  Can rhesus monkeys spontaneously subtract? , 2001, Cognition.

[31]  W A Roberts,et al.  Summation of symbols by pigeons (Columba livia): the importance of number and mass of reward items. , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[32]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Number Versus Contour Length in Infants' Discrimination of Small Visual Sets , 1999 .

[33]  R M Church,et al.  Temporal integration in duration and number discrimination. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[34]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  K. Hiraki,et al.  Baby arithmetic: one object plus one tone , 2004, Cognition.

[36]  R. Gelman,et al.  Young children can add and subtract by predicting and checking , 2004 .

[37]  S. Carey,et al.  The Representations Underlying Infants' Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes , 2002, Psychological science.

[38]  Karen C. Fuson,et al.  More Complexities in Subtraction. , 1984 .