Crossing the Pillars of Hercules: The Role of Spatial Frames of Reference in Error Making
暂无分享,去创建一个
Fabio Ferlazzo | Francesco Di Nocera | Alessandro Couyoumdjian | F. Ferlazzo | A. Couyoumdjian | Francesco Di Nocera
[1] Alan Cowey,et al. Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[2] F. Previc. The neuropsychology of 3-D space. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.
[3] A. Wohlschläger,et al. Mental and manual rotation. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .
[5] Klaus Hinkelmann,et al. Design and Analysis of Experiments: Introduction to Experimental Design , 1994 .
[6] O. Grüsser,et al. Multimodal Structure of the Extrapersonal Space , 1983 .
[7] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[8] Ma Conway,et al. Handbook of perception and cognition , 1996 .
[9] A. Wohlschläger,et al. Visual motion priming by invisible actions , 2000, Vision Research.
[10] W. Prinz,et al. The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .
[11] A. Treisman,et al. Attention, Space, and Action: Studies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .
[12] Kenneth M. Heilman,et al. The Influence of Center of Mass Effect on the Distribution of Spatial Attention in the Vertical and Horizontal Dimensions , 1997, Brain and Cognition.
[13] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[14] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Neural Circuits for Spatial Attention and Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .
[15] G. Rizzolatti,et al. From mirror neurons to imitation: Facts and speculations , 2002 .
[16] Yves Rossetti,et al. Implicit Short-Lived Motor Representations of Space in Brain Damaged and Healthy Subjects , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.
[17] A. Cowey,et al. No abrupt change in visual hemineglect from near to far space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[18] A. Berti,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[19] Y. Coello,et al. Retinal and extra-retinal contribution to position coding , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.
[20] G. Kerkhoff. Multiple perceptual distortions and their modulation in leftsided visual neglect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[21] M. Jeannerod. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .
[22] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Parietal cortex: from sight to action , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[23] A. Wohlschläger. Mental object rotation and the planning of hand movements , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] D. Norman. Categorization of action slips. , 1981 .
[25] J. Brožek. Attention and Performance II. , 1971 .
[26] J. Duncan. Converging levels of analysis in the cognitive neuroscience of visual attention. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[27] M A Arbib,et al. Schemas for the temporal organization of behaviour. , 1985, Human neurobiology.
[28] Margaret J. Robertson,et al. Design and Analysis of Experiments , 2006, Handbook of statistics.
[29] Pierluigi Zoccolotti,et al. Influence of the radial and vertical dimensions on lateral neglect , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[30] James E. Cutting,et al. Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .
[31] Stefano Cappa,et al. Visual Neglect for Far and Near Extra-Personal Space in Humans , 1989, Cortex.
[32] K. Fernow. New York , 1896, American Potato Journal.
[33] Yann Coello,et al. The Contribution of Retinal Signal to the Specification of Target Distance in a Visuo-manual Task. , 2000 .
[34] S. Tipper,et al. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.
[35] A. Cohen,et al. Action-based and vision-based selection of input: Two sources of control , 2002, Psychological research.
[36] Steven M. Finbeiner,et al. The Neural and Behavioral Organization of Goal-Directed Movements , 1989, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.
[37] B. Bridgeman,et al. Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] G. Ishimura. Visuomotor factors for action capture , 1995 .
[39] Gereon R. Fink,et al. Spatial Cognition: Where We Were and Where We Are , 2001, NeuroImage.
[40] Gereon R Fink,et al. Are action and perception in near and far space additive or interactive factors? , 2003, NeuroImage.
[41] F. Ferlazzo,et al. Functional Representation of 3d Space in Endogenous Attention Shifts , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[42] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[43] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Visuomotor neurons: ambiguity of the discharge or 'motor' perception? , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[44] A. Pouget,et al. Relating unilateral neglect to the neural coding of space , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[45] E. Sadalla,et al. Reference points in spatial cognition. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[46] T Landis,et al. So near yet so far: Neglect in far or near space depends on tool use , 2001, Annals of neurology.
[47] Jordan Grafman,et al. Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .
[48] M. Jeannerod. The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.
[49] David E. Irwin,et al. Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above? , 1993, Cognition.