Using repeated ingestion to determine the effect of sweetness, viscosity and oiliness on temporal perception of soymilk astringency

Abstract Astringency is a persistent sensation which increases upon repeated ingestion. To evaluate the effect of viscosity, sucrose and oil on perception of astringency during consumption of soymilk, a sequential sipping time–intensity (TI) procedure was utilized. For each soymilk, judges sipped the first of four ingestions and initiated the continuous recording of astringency intensity. Each sip was expectorated at 10 s after ingestion, and sipped 10 s after expectoration of the previous stimulus. After the fourth sample, judges rated astringency for 30 s. Traditional TI parameters, as well as rate of onset for each sip and increase in maximum intensity per sip were extracted from the TI curves. Maximum astringency (IMAX) increased significantly with successive sips as did the astringency at the time of sipping. Time to IMAX decreased from sip 1 to 3, but was longer for sip 4, which may be an artifact of the rapid test pace. Although addition of 60 g l −1 canola oil had no affect on astringency, adding 40 g l −1 sucrose or increasing viscosity by 5 cp with CMC significantly lowered all astringency parameters. The reduction in astringency by CMC may result from restoration of salivary lubrication and in part by chelation or hydrogen bonding of CMC to the astringents reducing their ability to bind to salivary proteins. The reduction in astringency produced by sucrose is more probably due to a cognitive process. ©

[1]  A. Noble,et al.  Effects of increased viscosity on the sourness and astringency of aluminum sulfate and citric acid , 1998 .

[2]  Pascal Schlich,et al.  Uses of change-over designs and repeated measurements in sensory and consumer studies , 1993 .

[3]  L. Butler,et al.  The specificity of proanthocyanidin-protein interactions. , 1981, The Journal of biological chemistry.

[4]  Ann C. Noble,et al.  Temporal perception of astringency and sweetness in red wine , 1995 .

[5]  M. Williamson,et al.  Polyphenols, astringency and proline-rich proteins. , 1994, Phytochemistry.

[6]  B. J. Lyman,et al.  Oral astringency: effects of repeated exposure and interactions with sweeteners , 1990 .

[7]  A. Noble,et al.  Effects of viscosity on the bitterness and astringency of grape seed tannin , 1996 .

[8]  M. Ono,et al.  Reduction of persimmon astringency by complex formation between pectin and tannins , 1997 .

[9]  P. Sherman,et al.  Identification of Stimuli Controlling the Sensory Evaluation of Viscosity. I : Non-Oral Methods , 1973 .

[10]  B. Green,et al.  Psychophysical evidence that oral astringency is a tactile sensation , 1993 .

[11]  R. Pangborn,et al.  The Time-Course of Astringency in Wine upon Repeated Ingestion , 1986 .

[12]  Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al.  Sweetness of sucrose, neohesperidin dihydrochalcone, and saccharin is related to genetic ability to taste the bitter substance 6-n-propylthiouracil , 1983 .

[13]  A. Noble,et al.  Effect of sweetener type and of sweetener and acid levels on temporal perception of sweetness, sourness and fruitiness , 1993 .

[14]  S. Taira Reduction of astringency in persimmon caused by adhesion of tannins to cell wall fragments , 1997 .

[15]  H. Snyder,et al.  Detection and Control of Soymilk Astringency , 1983 .

[16]  M. W. Kearsley,et al.  Saponin content of soya and some commercial soya products by means of high‐performance liquid chromatography of the sapogenins , 1986 .