Attention shifting in Parkinson's disease: an analysis of behavioral and cortical responses.
暂无分享,去创建一个
B. Kopp | M. Wittfoth | D. Dressler | C. Schrader | R. Dengler | R. Rodriguez-Raecke | L. Timm | D. Agrawal | F. Wegner | P. Tacik | N. Rustamov
[1] C. Fiebach,et al. Dissociable fronto-striatal effects of dopamine D2 receptor stimulation on cognitive versus motor flexibility , 2013, Cortex.
[2] Valerie Voon,et al. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease: decreased striatal dopamine transporter levels , 2013, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.
[3] Megan M. Risi,et al. Frontal and posterior subtypes of neuropsychological deficit in Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Behavioral neuroscience.
[4] P. Redgrave,et al. Functional properties of the basal ganglia's re-entrant loop architecture: selection and reinforcement , 2011, Neuroscience.
[5] D. Burn,et al. Parkinson's disease: The quintessential neuropsychiatric disorder , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[6] T. Anderson,et al. Characterizing mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[7] Michael Falkenstein,et al. Independent component analysis of erroneous and correct responses suggests online response control , 2010, Human brain mapping.
[8] Douglas P. Munoz,et al. Executive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Response automaticity and task switching , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[9] M. Schwarz,et al. Response Monitoring in De Novo Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2009, PloS one.
[10] M. Falkenstein,et al. Effects of stimulus–response compatibility on inhibitory processes in Parkinson’s disease , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[11] L. Nyberg,et al. Temporal dynamics of basal ganglia under-recruitment in Parkinson's disease: transient caudate abnormalities during updating of working memory. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[12] Pasquale Calabrese,et al. Screening for cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease with the Parkinson neuropsychometric dementia assessment (PANDA) instrument , 2008 .
[13] M. Schwarz,et al. Error processing in patients with Parkinson’s disease: the influence of medication state , 2008, Journal of Neural Transmission.
[14] A. Owen,et al. Attentional control in Parkinson's disease is dependent on COMT val 158 met genotype. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[15] S. Segalowitz,et al. The error negativity in nonmedicated and medicated patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[16] T. Robbins. Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[17] F. J. Friedrich,et al. Flanker compatibility effects in patients with Parkinson’s disease: Impact of target onset delay and trial-by-trial stimulus variation , 2007, Brain and Cognition.
[18] M. Petrides,et al. Cortical activity in Parkinson's disease during executive processing depends on striatal involvement. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[19] R. O’Reilly. Biologically Based Computational Models of High-Level Cognition , 2006, Science.
[20] Francisco Barceló,et al. Task Switching and Novelty Processing Activate a Common Neural Network for Cognitive Control , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] Michael J. Frank,et al. A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.
[22] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Effects of stimulus-response compatibility in Parkinson’s disease: a psychophysiological analysis , 2006, Journal of Neural Transmission.
[23] L. Nystrom,et al. Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] N. Georgiou-Karistianis,et al. Temporal variation in the control of goal-directed visuospatial attention in basal ganglia disorders , 2006, Neuroscience Research.
[25] T. Robbins,et al. Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[26] A. Engel,et al. Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] A. Kleinschmidt,et al. The attentional field has a Mexican hat distribution , 2005, Vision Research.
[28] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.
[29] Peter Praamstra,et al. The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[30] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.
[31] Peter Redgrave,et al. A computational model of action selection in the basal ganglia. I. A new functional anatomy , 2001, Biological Cybernetics.
[32] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Action monitoring, error detection, and the basal ganglia: an ERP study , 2001, Neuroreport.
[33] M. J. Emerson,et al. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[34] Peter Praamstra,et al. Motor cortex activation in Parkinson's disease: Dissociation of electrocortical and peripheral measures of response generation , 1999, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[35] L. Defebvre,et al. Impairment of the supervisory attentional system in early untreated patients with Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Journal of Neurology.
[36] S. Keele,et al. Toward a Functional Analysis of the Basal Ganglia , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] D. Meyer,et al. A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .
[38] T. Robbins,et al. Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[39] C. Sherbourne,et al. The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) , 1992 .
[40] E. A. Berg,et al. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. , 1948, The Journal of general psychology.
[41] M D'Esposito,et al. Enhanced frontal function in Parkinson's disease. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[42] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. On the Control of Control: The Role of Dopamine in Regulating Prefrontal Function and Working Memory , 2007 .
[43] D F Stegeman,et al. Reliance on external cues for movement initiation in Parkinson's disease. Evidence from movement-related potentials. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[44] C. Marsden,et al. An investigation of the phenomenon of “set” in Parkinson's disease , 1988, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[45] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[46] O. Hikosaka,et al. Perceptual Learning, Motor Learning and Automaticity Switching from Automatic to Controlled Behavior: Cortico-basal Ganglia Mechanisms , 2022 .