Attention shifting in Parkinson's disease: an analysis of behavioral and cortical responses.

OBJECTIVE The study was designed to examine persistent (input selection) versus transient (input shifting) mechanisms of attention control in Parkinson's disease (PD). METHOD The study identifies behavioral and neural markers of selective control and shifting control using a novel combination of a flanker task with an attentional set-shifting task, and it compares patients with PD with matched controls. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded, and analyses focused on frontally distributed N2 waves, parietally distributed P3 waves, and error-related negativities (Ne/ERN). RESULTS Controls showed robust shifting costs (prolonged response times), but patients with PD did not show evidence for comparable shifting costs. Patients with PD made more errors than controls when required to shift between attentional sets, but also when they had to initially maintain an attentional set. At the neural level it was found that contrary to controls, patients with PD did not display any N2 and P3 augmentations on shift trials. Patients with PD further did not display any error-related activity or posterror N2 augmentation. CONCLUSIONS Our results reveal that intact selective control and disrupted shifting control are dissociable in patients with PD, but additional work is required to dissect the proportionate effects of disease and treatment on shifting control in PD. Our ERP-based approach opens a new window onto an understanding of motor and cognitive flexibility that seems to be associated with the dopaminergic innervation of cortico-striatal loops.

[1]  C. Fiebach,et al.  Dissociable fronto-striatal effects of dopamine D2 receptor stimulation on cognitive versus motor flexibility , 2013, Cortex.

[2]  Valerie Voon,et al.  Impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease: decreased striatal dopamine transporter levels , 2013, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[3]  Megan M. Risi,et al.  Frontal and posterior subtypes of neuropsychological deficit in Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Behavioral neuroscience.

[4]  P. Redgrave,et al.  Functional properties of the basal ganglia's re-entrant loop architecture: selection and reinforcement , 2011, Neuroscience.

[5]  D. Burn,et al.  Parkinson's disease: The quintessential neuropsychiatric disorder , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[6]  T. Anderson,et al.  Characterizing mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[7]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Independent component analysis of erroneous and correct responses suggests online response control , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[8]  Douglas P. Munoz,et al.  Executive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Response automaticity and task switching , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  M. Schwarz,et al.  Response Monitoring in De Novo Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2009, PloS one.

[10]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Effects of stimulus–response compatibility on inhibitory processes in Parkinson’s disease , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[11]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Temporal dynamics of basal ganglia under-recruitment in Parkinson's disease: transient caudate abnormalities during updating of working memory. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Pasquale Calabrese,et al.  Screening for cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease with the Parkinson neuropsychometric dementia assessment (PANDA) instrument , 2008 .

[13]  M. Schwarz,et al.  Error processing in patients with Parkinson’s disease: the influence of medication state , 2008, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[14]  A. Owen,et al.  Attentional control in Parkinson's disease is dependent on COMT val 158 met genotype. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  The error negativity in nonmedicated and medicated patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[16]  T. Robbins Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Flanker compatibility effects in patients with Parkinson’s disease: Impact of target onset delay and trial-by-trial stimulus variation , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[18]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cortical activity in Parkinson's disease during executive processing depends on striatal involvement. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  R. O’Reilly Biologically Based Computational Models of High-Level Cognition , 2006, Science.

[20]  Francisco Barceló,et al.  Task Switching and Novelty Processing Activate a Common Neural Network for Cognitive Control , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of stimulus-response compatibility in Parkinson’s disease: a psychophysiological analysis , 2006, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[23]  L. Nystrom,et al.  Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  N. Georgiou-Karistianis,et al.  Temporal variation in the control of goal-directed visuospatial attention in basal ganglia disorders , 2006, Neuroscience Research.

[25]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Kleinschmidt,et al.  The attentional field has a Mexican hat distribution , 2005, Vision Research.

[28]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[29]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[31]  Peter Redgrave,et al.  A computational model of action selection in the basal ganglia. I. A new functional anatomy , 2001, Biological Cybernetics.

[32]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Action monitoring, error detection, and the basal ganglia: an ERP study , 2001, Neuroreport.

[33]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  Motor cortex activation in Parkinson's disease: Dissociation of electrocortical and peripheral measures of response generation , 1999, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[35]  L. Defebvre,et al.  Impairment of the supervisory attentional system in early untreated patients with Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Journal of Neurology.

[36]  S. Keele,et al.  Toward a Functional Analysis of the Basal Ganglia , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[38]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  C. Sherbourne,et al.  The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) , 1992 .

[40]  E. A. Berg,et al.  A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. , 1948, The Journal of general psychology.

[41]  M D'Esposito,et al.  Enhanced frontal function in Parkinson's disease. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  On the Control of Control: The Role of Dopamine in Regulating Prefrontal Function and Working Memory , 2007 .

[43]  D F Stegeman,et al.  Reliance on external cues for movement initiation in Parkinson's disease. Evidence from movement-related potentials. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  C. Marsden,et al.  An investigation of the phenomenon of “set” in Parkinson's disease , 1988, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[45]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[46]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Perceptual Learning, Motor Learning and Automaticity Switching from Automatic to Controlled Behavior: Cortico-basal Ganglia Mechanisms , 2022 .