Contributions of implicit and explicit memories to sensorimotor adaptation of movement extent during goal-directed reaching
暂无分享,去创建一个
Robert A Scheidt | Leigh Ann Mrotek | Devon D Lantagne | Rebecca A Slick | Scott A Beardsley | Danny G Thomas | R. Scheidt | S. Beardsley | L. A. Mrotek | R. Slick | D. Thomas
[1] Gordon M. Redding,et al. Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[2] L. Squire,et al. Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. , 1994, Learning & memory.
[3] P. Viviani,et al. Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] C Ghez,et al. Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[5] C. Prablanc,et al. Automatic Drive of Limb Motor Plasticity , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] Howard Eichenbaum,et al. What H.M. Taught Us , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[8] R. Shadmehr,et al. A Shared Resource between Declarative Memory and Motor Memory , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Yves Rossetti,et al. Enhancing Visuomotor Adaptation by Reducing Error Signals: Single-step (Aware) versus Multiple-step (Unaware) Exposure to Wedge Prisms , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[10] J. Krakauer,et al. Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[11] R A Scheidt,et al. Learning to move amid uncertainty. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] O. Bock,et al. Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] J. Krakauer,et al. An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14] R. Shadmehr,et al. Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.
[15] C. Ghez,et al. Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] Otmar Bock,et al. Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.
[17] N. Schweighofer,et al. Dual Adaptation Supports a Parallel Architecture of Motor Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] S. Corkin. Lasting Consequences of Bilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy: Clinical Course and Experimental Findings in H.M. , 1984 .
[19] T. Judkins,et al. Visuo-proprioceptive interactions during adaptation of the human reach. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[21] Claude Ghez,et al. Separate adaptive mechanisms for controlling trajectory and final position in reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] J. Wann,et al. Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[23] L. Squire,et al. Encapsulation of Implicit and Explicit Memory in Sequence Learning , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.
[25] J. Lackner,et al. Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[26] Peter A. Frensch,et al. One concept, multiple meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit learning. , 1998 .
[27] Jordan A Taylor,et al. Explicit and Implicit Processes Constitute the Fast and Slow Processes of Sensorimotor Learning , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] C Ghez,et al. Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[29] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Motor Adaptation as a Process of Reoptimization , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[30] D. M. MacKay,et al. Source density analysis of scalp potentials during linguistic and non-linguistic processing of visual stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Mathias Hegele,et al. Explicit and implicit components of visuo-motor adaptation: An analysis of individual differences , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.
[32] M. Latash,et al. Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[33] C. Ghez,et al. Patterns of hypermetria and terminal cocontraction during point-to-point movements demonstrate independent action of trajectory and postural controllers. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] R B Bhat,et al. Cognitive Channels Computing Action Distance and Direction , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[35] T. Brashers-Krug,et al. Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] Alaa A. Ahmed,et al. Vigor of reaching movements: reward discounts the cost of effort. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.
[37] Carolee J Winstein,et al. Impact of explicit information on implicit motor-sequence learning following middle cerebral artery stroke. , 2003, Physical therapy.
[38] R A Scheidt,et al. Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[39] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] Joseph H. Solomon,et al. The Morphology of the Rat Vibrissal Array: A Model for Quantifying Spatiotemporal Patterns of Whisker-Object Contact , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[41] Joaquin A. Anguera,et al. A spatial explicit strategy reduces error but interferes with sensorimotor adaptation. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.