Contributions of implicit and explicit memories to sensorimotor adaptation of movement extent during goal-directed reaching

We examined how explicit memory of kinematic performance contributes to motor adaptation during goal-directed reaching. Twenty subjects grasped the handle of a horizontal planar robot that rendered spring-like resistance to movement. Subjects made rapid “out-and-back” reaches to capture a remembered visual target at the point of maximal reach extent. The robot’s resistance changed unpredictably between reaches, inducing target capture errors that subjects attempted to correct from one trial to the next. The subjects performed four sets of trials. Three were performed without concurrent visual cursor feedback of hand motion. Two of these required self-assessment of performance between trials, whereby subjects reported peak reach extent on the most recent trial. This was done by either moving a cursor on a horizontal display (visual self-assessment), or by moving the robot’s handle back to the recalled location (proprioceptive self-assessment). Two control conditions were performed either without or with concurrent visual cursor feedback of hand motion. We analyzed movement kinematics and used regression analyses to quantify the extent to which prior reach errors and explicit memories of prior performance contribute to subsequent reach performance. Consistent with prior reports, providing concurrent visual feedback of hand motion increased reach accuracy and reduced the impact of past performance errors on future performance, relative to the no-vision control condition. By contrast, we found no impact of interposed self-assessment on reach kinematics or on how prior target capture errors influence subsequent reach performance. Self-assessments were biased toward the remembered target location, differing markedly from actual reach performances. Including self-assessments as explicit memory of performance in the regression analyses did not improve model predictive accuracy. Therefore, we conclude that explicit memories of prior kinematic performance do not contribute to implicit sensorimotor adaptation of movement extent.

[1]  Gordon M. Redding,et al.  Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[2]  L. Squire,et al.  Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[3]  P. Viviani,et al.  Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[5]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic Drive of Limb Motor Plasticity , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Howard Eichenbaum,et al.  What H.M. Taught Us , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[8]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  A Shared Resource between Declarative Memory and Motor Memory , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Enhancing Visuomotor Adaptation by Reducing Error Signals: Single-step (Aware) versus Multiple-step (Unaware) Exposure to Wedge Prisms , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Learning to move amid uncertainty. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  O. Bock,et al.  Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[15]  C. Ghez,et al.  Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  N. Schweighofer,et al.  Dual Adaptation Supports a Parallel Architecture of Motor Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  S. Corkin Lasting Consequences of Bilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy: Clinical Course and Experimental Findings in H.M. , 1984 .

[19]  T. Judkins,et al.  Visuo-proprioceptive interactions during adaptation of the human reach. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  Claude Ghez,et al.  Separate adaptive mechanisms for controlling trajectory and final position in reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Wann,et al.  Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  L. Squire,et al.  Encapsulation of Implicit and Explicit Memory in Sequence Learning , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.

[25]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Peter A. Frensch,et al.  One concept, multiple meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit learning. , 1998 .

[27]  Jordan A Taylor,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Processes Constitute the Fast and Slow Processes of Sensorimotor Learning , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Motor Adaptation as a Process of Reoptimization , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  D. M. MacKay,et al.  Source density analysis of scalp potentials during linguistic and non-linguistic processing of visual stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Explicit and implicit components of visuo-motor adaptation: An analysis of individual differences , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[32]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  C. Ghez,et al.  Patterns of hypermetria and terminal cocontraction during point-to-point movements demonstrate independent action of trajectory and postural controllers. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  R B Bhat,et al.  Cognitive Channels Computing Action Distance and Direction , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Alaa A. Ahmed,et al.  Vigor of reaching movements: reward discounts the cost of effort. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Carolee J Winstein,et al.  Impact of explicit information on implicit motor-sequence learning following middle cerebral artery stroke. , 2003, Physical therapy.

[38]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Joseph H. Solomon,et al.  The Morphology of the Rat Vibrissal Array: A Model for Quantifying Spatiotemporal Patterns of Whisker-Object Contact , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[41]  Joaquin A. Anguera,et al.  A spatial explicit strategy reduces error but interferes with sensorimotor adaptation. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.