Hierarchical control and skilled typing: evidence for word-level control over the execution of individual keystrokes.

Routine actions are commonly assumed to be controlled by hierarchically organized processes and representations. In the domain of typing theories, word-level information is assumed to activate the constituent keystrokes required to type each letter in a word. We tested this assumption directly using a novel single-letter probe technique. Subjects were primed with a visual or auditory word or a visually presented random consonant string and then probed to type a single letter from the prime or another randomly selected letter. Relative to randomly selected letters, probe responses were speeded for first, middle, and last letters contained in visual and auditory word primes but not for middle and last letters contained in random consonant primes. This suggests that word-level information causes parallel activation of constituent keystrokes, consistent with hierarchical processing. The role of hierarchical processing in typing and routine action is discussed.

[1]  William E. Cooper,et al.  Cognitive Aspects of Skilled Typewriting , 2011, Springer New York.

[2]  Matthew J. C. Crump,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Left Hand Doesn’t Know What the Right Hand Is Doing The Disruptive Effects of Attention to the Hands in Skilled , 2022 .

[3]  K. Rayner,et al.  Language processing in reading and speech perception is fast and incremental: Implications for event-related potential research , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[4]  Yili Liu,et al.  Queuing Network Modeling of Transcription Typing , 2008, TCHI.

[5]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Retrieving information from a hierarchical plan. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  M. Rieger Letters as visual action-effects in skilled typing. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[7]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: switching tasks in sequences. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  T. McNamara Semantic Priming: Perspectives from Memory and Word Recognition , 2005 .

[9]  Thomas A. Schreiber,et al.  The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[10]  M. Rieger Automatic keypress activation in skilled typing. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  D. Plaut,et al.  Doing without schema hierarchies: a recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. , 2004, Psychological review.

[12]  G. Logan Simon-type effects: chronometric evidence for keypress schemata in typewriting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  T. Shallice,et al.  CONTENTION SCHEDULING AND THE CONTROL OF ROUTINE ACTIVITIES , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[14]  Bonnie E. John TYPIST: A Theory of Performance in Skilled Typing , 1996, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[15]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  A Solution to the Effect of Sample Size on Outlier Elimination , 1994 .

[17]  Alan J. Dix,et al.  Human-Computer Interaction , 1993, Encyclopedia of Database Systems.

[18]  J F Soechting,et al.  Kinematics of typing: parallel control of the two hands. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  T. Salthouse Perceptual, cognitive, and motoric aspects of transcription typing. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Tutorials in Motor Behavior , 1980 .

[21]  Jonathan Grudin,et al.  Finger Movements in Transcription Typing , 1980 .

[22]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[23]  L. Shaffer Intention and performance. , 1976 .

[24]  L. H. Shaffer,et al.  Control Processes in Typing , 1975 .

[25]  S. Jay Samuels,et al.  Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading , 1974 .

[26]  G. M. Reicher Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[27]  L. Shaffer,et al.  Typing Performance as a Function of Text , 1968 .

[28]  G. Miller,et al.  Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .

[29]  Victor S Ferreira,et al.  Language production. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[30]  Udo Will,et al.  Linguistic units in word typing: Effects of word presentation modes and typing delay , 2006 .

[31]  Udo Will,et al.  Morphemes, syllables and graphemes in written word production , 2004 .

[32]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Stroop-Type Interference : Congruity Effects in Color Naming With Typewritten Responses , 1998 .

[33]  G. Dell,et al.  Language production and serial order: a functional analysis and a model. , 1997, Psychological review.

[34]  Serge Larochelle,et al.  A Comparison of Skilled and Novice Performance in Discontinuous Typing , 1983 .

[35]  D. Gentner Keystroke Timing in Transcription Typing , 1983 .

[36]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Simulating a Skilled Typist: A Study of Skilled Cognitive-Motor Performance , 1982, Cogn. Sci..

[37]  D. Ostry 27 Execution-Time Movement Control , 1980 .

[38]  G. Stelmach Information processing in motor control and learning , 1978 .

[39]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[40]  L. A. Jeffress,et al.  Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior , 1953 .

[41]  K. Lashley The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .