Valence-specific regulation effects in a working memory task with emotional context

The modulatory effects of emotional context on episodic memory have been shown recently. We were interested whether working memory is similarly susceptible to emotional context or whether working memory helps to distract from emotion, i.e. serves to downregulate emotional reactions in order to sustain the prior intention. We tested the effects of emotional stimulation during active maintenance of information by modulating the emotional context of maintenance processes in a Sternberg item recognition task with two load conditions. Behavioral data revealed no impairment of working memory performance during emotional context. Actually, behavioral performance was better for emotional compared to neutral context during high load. Furthermore, emotional context had no interference effect on working memory-related brain activation. Instead, we found a valence-specific regulation effect: High cognitive effort was associated with reduced activity in emotion processing regions, i.e. the amygdala and ventral striatum. This effect was mediated by different prefrontal regions, i.e. by left inferior PFC for negative and left superior PFC for positive valence. Furthermore, our results reveal an integration effect of emotion and cognition in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex showing increased recruitment with increasing complexity of the task. The results presented here are of relevance for the understanding of regulatory mechanisms and diseases characterized by increased susceptibility to emotional distraction.

[1]  R. Davidson,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Richard N A Henson,et al.  Modulation of retrieval processing reflects accuracy of emotional source memory. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[3]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[5]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Cognitive modulation of emotion anticipation , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain , 2004, Nature.

[7]  J. Gray,et al.  Emotional modulation of cognitive control: approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  P. Lang International Affective Picture System (IAPS) : Technical Manual and Affective Ratings , 1995 .

[9]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Imaging Attentional Modulation of Pain in the Periaqueductal Gray in Humans , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Panksepp,et al.  The neurobiology of positive emotions , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[11]  A. Baddeley The central executive: A concept and some misconceptions , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[12]  M. Raichle,et al.  Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Rethinking Feelings: An fMRI Study of the Cognitive Regulation of Emotion , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Yue-Jia Luo,et al.  Differential influences of negative emotion on spatial and verbal working memory: evidence from event-related potential and source current density analysis , 2006, Neuroreport.

[15]  S. Clare,et al.  Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  H. Damasio Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images , 1995 .

[17]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Emotional context during encoding of neutral items modulates brain activation not only during encoding but also during recognition , 2005, NeuroImage.

[18]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Repetition suppression of faces is modulated by emotion. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  R. Knight,et al.  Human prefrontal lesions increase distractibility to irrelevant sensory inputs , 1995, Neuroreport.

[20]  Terri Gullickson Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images. , 1995 .

[21]  Till Sprenger,et al.  Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain—an fMRI analysis , 2004, Pain.

[22]  Arthur P. Wunderlich,et al.  Emotional context modulates subsequent memory effect , 2003, NeuroImage.

[23]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Affective and cognitive modulation of performance monitoring: Behavioral and ERP evidence , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[24]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[25]  A. P. R Smith,et al.  fMRI correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts , 2004, NeuroImage.

[26]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Attentional control of the processing of neural and emotional stimuli. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[27]  L. Cahill Why sex matters for neuroscience , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[28]  Derek G. V. Mitchell,et al.  The impact of processing load on emotion , 2007, NeuroImage.

[29]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  B. Postle Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain , 2006, Neuroscience.

[31]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[32]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[33]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Individual Differences in Amygdala Activity Predict Response Speed during Working Memory , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  R. Henson,et al.  Neural activity associated with episodic memory for emotional context , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  Jeffrey S. Maxwell,et al.  Anxiety selectively disrupts visuospatial working memory. , 2006, Emotion.

[36]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  Florin Dolcos,et al.  Role of the inferior frontal cortex in coping with distracting emotions , 2006, Neuroreport.

[38]  Daniel V Meegan,et al.  Task analysis complements neuroimaging: an example from working memory research , 2004, NeuroImage.

[39]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence of a Right Hemisphere Bias for the Influence of Negative Emotion on Higher Cognition , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Evaluation of a novel event-related parametric fMRI paradigm investigating prefrontal function , 2005, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[41]  P. Fox,et al.  Computational approaches to network analysis in functional brain imaging , 1994 .

[42]  Jeremy R. Gray,et al.  Threat-Evoked Anxiety Disrupts Spatial Working Memory Performance: An Attentional Account , 2003, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[43]  Jennifer C. Britton,et al.  Neural correlates of social and nonsocial emotions: An fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[44]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Brain Systems Mediating Cognitive Interference by Emotional Distraction , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion , 2004, NeuroImage.

[46]  W. Richter,et al.  Distraction Modulates Anterior Cingulate Gyrus Activations during the Cold Pressor Test , 2001, NeuroImage.

[47]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  Human Striatal Responses to Monetary Reward Depend On Saliency , 2004, Neuron.