The Influence of Incidental Affect on Consumers’ Food Intake

Although incidental affect has been shown to influence both attitude and purchase behavior, it has not been extended to actual consumption. This research investigates whether specific affective states influence food consumption and whether this influence is moderated by factors such as information and the nature of the product (hedonic versus less hedonic). The authors show that an integrative mood management and mood evaluation framework accounts for this relationship more effectively than a self-regulation explanation. A preliminary test and two lab studies show that people eat larger amounts of hedonic foods (buttered popcorn and M&M's) when they are in a sad state than when they are in a happy state and that this effect is attenuated when nutritional information is present. In contrast, they tend to eat larger amounts of a less hedonic product (raisins) when they are in a happy state than when they are in a sad state. The authors discuss implications for responsible marketers, health professionals, and health conscious consumers in the context of campaigns and individual efforts.

[1]  D. L. Call,et al.  Consumer Reaction to Nutritional Labels on Food Products , 1973 .

[2]  S. Turell,et al.  Desirable and undesirable masculine and feminine traits in relation to students' dieting tendencies and body image dissatisfaction , 1983 .

[3]  Gary J. Russell,et al.  Nutrition Information in the Supermarket , 1986 .

[4]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  C. Moorman The Effects of Stimulus and Consumer Characteristics on the Utilization of Nutrition Information , 1990 .

[6]  David Glen Mick,et al.  Self-Gifts: Phenomenological Insights from Four Contexts , 1990 .

[7]  John D. Mayer,et al.  Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. , 1992 .

[8]  M. Hetherington,et al.  "Chocolate Addiction": a Preliminary Study of its Description and its Relationship to Problem Eating , 1993, Appetite.

[9]  R. Baumeister,et al.  When ego threats lead to self-regulation failure: negative consequences of high self-esteem. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  R. Wing,et al.  Stress-induced eating. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  B. Wansink Can Package Size Accelerate Usage Volume? , 1995 .

[12]  Christine Moorman,et al.  A Quasi Experiment to Assess the Consumer and Informational Determinants of Nutrition Information Processing Activities: The Case of the Nutrition Labeling and Education Act , 1996 .

[13]  B. Wansink,et al.  Advertising Strategies to Increase Usage Frequency , 1996 .

[14]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  S. Siddarth,et al.  The Last Straw? Cigarette Advertising and Realized Market Shares among Youths and Adults, 1979–1993 , 1996 .

[16]  H. Woodruffe,et al.  Compensatory consumption: why women go shopping when they’re fed up and other stories , 1997 .

[17]  Scott A. Neslin,et al.  The Effect of Promotion on Consumption: Buying More and Consuming it Faster , 1998 .

[18]  M. Rothschild,et al.  Carrots, Sticks, and Promises: A Conceptual Framework for the Management of Public Health and Social Issue Behaviors , 2000 .

[19]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  Beyond valence in the perception of likelihood: the role of emotion specificity. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  George Baltas,et al.  The Effects of Nutrition Information on Consumer Choice , 2001, Journal of Advertising Research.

[21]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  J. Lerner,et al.  Fear, anger, and risk. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  G. Fitzsimons,et al.  Nonconscious and Contaminative Effects of Hypothetical Questions on Subsequent Decision Making , 2001 .

[24]  J. Jeffrey Inman,et al.  The Role of Sensory‐Specific Satiety in Attribute‐Level Variety Seeking , 2001 .

[25]  Catherine A. Cole,et al.  Consumers' Search and Use of Nutrition Information: The Challenge and Promise of the Nutrition Labeling and Education Act , 2002 .

[26]  Paul Rozin,et al.  Food and life, pleasure and worry, among American college students: gender differences and regional similarities. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  Brian Wansink,et al.  Exploring comfort food preferences across age and gender , 2003, Physiology & Behavior.

[28]  Scot Burton,et al.  Making Healthful Food Choices: The Influence of Health Claims and Nutrition Information on Consumers’ Evaluations of Packaged Food Products and Restaurant Menu Items , 2003 .

[29]  Christine Moorman,et al.  Subjective Knowledge, Search Locations, and Consumer Choice , 2004 .

[30]  Michel Tuan Pham The Logic of Feeling , 2004 .

[31]  Brian Wansink,et al.  De-Marketing Obesity , 2005 .

[32]  Nitika Garg,et al.  Incidental and Task-Related Affect: A Re-Inquiry and Extension of the Influence of Affect on Choice , 2005 .

[33]  Eduardo B. Andrade Behavioral Consequences of Affect: Combining Evaluative and Regulatory Mechanisms , 2005 .