Intuition and Moral Decision-Making – The Effect of Time Pressure and Cognitive Load on Moral Judgment and Altruistic Behavior

Do individuals intuitively favor certain moral actions over others? This study explores the role of intuitive thinking—induced by time pressure and cognitive load—in moral judgment and behavior. We conduct experiments in three different countries (Sweden, Austria, and the United States) involving over 1,400 subjects. All subjects responded to four trolley type dilemmas and four dictator games involving different charitable causes. Decisions were made under time pressure/time delay or while experiencing cognitive load or control. Overall we find converging evidence that intuitive states do not influence moral decisions. Neither time-pressure nor cognitive load had any effect on moral judgments or altruistic behavior. Thus we find no supporting evidence for the claim that intuitive moral judgments and dictator game giving differ from more reflectively taken decisions. Across all samples and decision tasks men were more likely to make utilitarian moral judgments and act selfishly compared to women, providing further evidence that there are robust gender differences in moral decision-making. However, there were no significant interactions between gender and the treatment manipulations of intuitive versus reflective decision-making.

[1]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  A meta-analysis of response-time tests of the sequential two-systems model of moral judgment , 2017, Memory & cognition.

[2]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  Intuition and cooperation reconsidered , 2013, Nature.

[3]  Ralph Hertwig,et al.  Time and moral judgment , 2011, Cognition.

[4]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Use of a Rasch model to predict response times to utilitarian moral dilemmas , 2012, Synthese.

[5]  David G. Rand,et al.  Human cooperation , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Jean‐François Bonnefon,et al.  Efficient Kill–Save Ratios Ease Up the Cognitive Demands on Counterintuitive Moral Utilitarianism , 2014, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[7]  J. Piaget The Moral Judgment of the Child , 1932 .

[8]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[9]  David G. Rand,et al.  Spontaneous giving and calculated greed , 2012, Nature.

[10]  F. Youssef,et al.  Stress alters personal moral decision making , 2012, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[11]  Siegfried Dewitte,et al.  Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology Are Social Value Orientations Expressed Automatically? Decision Making in the Dictator Game on Behalf Of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology , 2022 .

[12]  D. Kahneman Thinking, Fast and Slow , 2011 .

[13]  Jean-Robert Tyran,et al.  Fairness is intuitive , 2014 .

[14]  Peter P. J. L. Verkoeijen,et al.  Does Intuition Cause Cooperation? , 2014, PloS one.

[15]  Daniel T. Gilbert,et al.  Thinking backward: some curable and incurable consequences of cognitive busyness , 1989 .

[16]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment , 2001, Science.

[17]  J. Savulescu,et al.  ‘Utilitarian’ judgments in sacrificial moral dilemmas do not reflect impartial concern for the greater good , 2015, Cognition.

[18]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Individual differences in rational thought. , 1998 .

[19]  David G. Rand,et al.  Social Heuristics and Social Roles: Intuition Favors Altruism for Women But Not for Men , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  Ernst Fehr,et al.  Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference , 2015, Nature Communications.

[21]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect , 2016, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[22]  Commentary: Fairness is intuitive , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[23]  C. Nathan DeWall,et al.  Violence restrained: Effects of self-regulation and its depletion on aggression , 2007 .

[24]  Clemens Puppe,et al.  Optimal Redistricting Under Geographical Constraints: Why 'Pack and Crack' Does Not Work , 2008 .

[25]  J. Thomson The Trolley Problem , 1985 .

[26]  C. Gilligan In a Different Voice. Psychological Theory and Women’s Development. Cambridge, MA (Harvard University Press) 1982. , 1982 .

[27]  M. P. Espinosa,et al.  Prosocial behavior and gender , 2015, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[28]  J. Haidt The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. , 2001, Psychological review.

[29]  Daniel T. Gilbert,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology of Thoughts Unspoken Social Inference and the Self-regulation of Behavior , 2022 .

[30]  Evan C. Carter,et al.  Publication bias and the limited strength model of self-control: has the evidence for ego depletion been overestimated? , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[31]  R. Byrne,et al.  Dual processes of emotion and reason in judgments about moral dilemmas , 2014 .

[32]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.

[33]  S. Epstein,et al.  Conflict Between Intuitive and Rational Processing: When People Behave Against Their Better Judgment , 1994 .

[34]  S. Frederick Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[35]  J. Baron,et al.  Challenges for the sequential two-system model of moral judgement , 2017 .

[36]  Andrew Gelman,et al.  Data-dependent analysis—a "garden of forking paths"— explains why many statistically significant comparisons don't hold up. , 2014 .

[37]  Dual Processes and Moral Rules , 2011 .

[38]  G. Tinghög The Art of Saying No The Economics and Ethics of Healthcare Rationing , 2011 .

[39]  P. Foot The Problem of Abortion and the Doctrine of the Double Effect , 2020, The Doctrine of Double Effect.

[40]  Eong Jinkyu,et al.  What is the Trolley Problem , 2015 .

[41]  S. Epstein Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. , 1994, The American psychologist.

[42]  C. Holstein Irreversible, Stepwise Sequence in the Development of Moral Judgment: A Longitudinal Study of Males and Females. , 1976 .

[43]  F. Cushman,et al.  The Psychology of Dilemmas and the Philosophy of Morality , 2009 .

[44]  A. Gelman,et al.  The statistical crisis in science , 2014 .

[45]  Jean‐François Bonnefon,et al.  Mortality salience and morality: Thinking about death makes people less utilitarian , 2012, Cognition.

[46]  David G. Rand,et al.  Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation , 2014, Nature Communications.

[47]  The effect of fast and slow decisions on risk taking , 2017, Journal of risk and uncertainty.

[48]  B. Shiv,et al.  Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision Making , 1999 .

[49]  S. Epstein,et al.  Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: when people behave against their better judgment. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  C. K. Mertz,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Numeracy and Decision Making , 2022 .

[51]  Evan C. Carter,et al.  A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[52]  M. Hagger,et al.  Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  G. Becker,et al.  The Economic Approach to Human Behavior , 1978 .

[54]  L. Kohlberg Essays On Moral Development , 1981 .

[55]  M. Crockett Models of morality , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  Christoph Engel,et al.  Dictator games: a meta study , 2010 .

[57]  C. Engel Dictator games: a meta study , 2010 .

[58]  Judd B. Kessler,et al.  Learning from (failed) replications: Cognitive load manipulations and charitable giving , 2014 .

[59]  Lars-Olof Johansson,et al.  Keeping others in our mind or in our heart? Distribution games under cognitive load , 2016 .

[60]  C. Gilligan In a different voice: Women's conceptions of self and of morality. , 1977 .

[61]  Don A. Moore,et al.  Self-Interest, Automaticity, and the Psychology of Conflict of Interest , 2004 .

[62]  Verena Utikal,et al.  Affect and fairness: Dictator games under cognitive load , 2014 .

[63]  Erik Wengström,et al.  Fast or Fair? A Study of Response Times , 2008 .

[64]  Olga Antonenko,et al.  Gender differences in neural mechanisms underlying moral sensitivity. , 2008, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[65]  Rachel T. A. Croson,et al.  Gender Differences in Preferences , 2009 .

[66]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment , 2008, Cognition.

[67]  Shaun Nichols,et al.  Moral dilemmas and moral rules , 2006, Cognition.

[68]  J. Hyde,et al.  Gender differences in moral orientation: a meta-analysis. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[69]  D. Cummins,et al.  Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[70]  Joseph M. Paxton,et al.  Reflection and Reasoning in Moral Judgment , 2022 .

[71]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Toward a Physiology of Dual-Process Reasoning and Judgment: Lemonade, Willpower, and Expensive Rule-Based Analysis , 2008, Psychological science.

[72]  Adam B. Moore,et al.  Who Shalt Not Kill? Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity, Executive Control, and Moral Judgment , 2008, Psychological science.

[73]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  Deontological and utilitarian inclinations in moral decision making: a process dissociation approach. , 2013, Journal of personality and social psychology.