Goal or movement? Action representation within the primary motor cortex

Although facilitation of the cortico‐spinal system during action observation is widely accepted, it remains controversial whether this facilitation reflects a replica of the observed movements or the goal of the observed motor acts. In the present transcranial magnetic stimulation study, we recorded motor evoked potentials from two hand muscles (first dorsal interosseous and abductor digiti minimi) while 22 healthy participants observed a hand reaching towards and grasping a bottle. To test for alternative coding levels (goal vs. movement), three relevant aspects were systematically manipulated: the type of observed movement (precision grip or whole hand grasping), situational context (bottle positioned in front of or behind a wall‐like barrier), and processing stage (transcranial magnetic stimulation pulse delivered at the onset of the movement or at the moment of contact between the fingers and the object). At movement onset, motor evoked potential responses reflected the program necessary to achieve the action goal within the situational context. During movement observation, however, the type of observed movement was taken into account and a transition towards a movement‐related modulation was observed. These results suggest that, rather than being exclusive alternatives, goal coding and movement coding may relate to different processing stages.

[1]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Making Mirrors: Premotor Cortex Stimulation Enhances Mirror and Counter-mirror Motor Facilitation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  S. Boniface,et al.  Magnetic brain stimulation with a double coil: the importance of coil orientation. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  T. Paus,et al.  Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2000, Neuroreport.

[4]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Representation of Goal and Movements without Overt Motor Behavior in the Human Motor Cortex: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  S. Rossi,et al.  Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[6]  U. Castiello,et al.  The transfer of motor functional strategies via action observation , 2012, Biology Letters.

[7]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. , 2007, Human movement science.

[8]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation , 2001, Neuroreport.

[9]  M. Hallett,et al.  Optimal Focal Transcranial Magnetic Activation of the Human Motor Cortex: Effects of Coil Orientation, Shape of the Induced Current Pulse, and Stimulus Intensity , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[10]  U. Castiello,et al.  From simulation to reciprocity: The case of complementary actions , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[11]  E. Wassermann Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[12]  Marco Iacoboni,et al.  Lateralization in motor facilitation during action observation: a TMS study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  World Medical Association (WMA),et al.  Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects , 2009, Journal of the Indian Medical Association.

[15]  Scott T. Grafton The cognitive neuroscience of prehension: recent developments , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Scott T. Grafton Embodied Cognition and the Simulation of Action to Understand Others , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Michela Romani,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: topographic mapping of the target muscle and influence of the onlooker's posture , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  Jason W. Osbourne Notes on the Use of Data Transformation. , 2002 .

[19]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  S. Swinnen,et al.  How are observed actions mapped to the observer’s motor system? Influence of posture and perspective , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Simulating the future of actions in the human corticospinal system. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Umberto Castiello,et al.  Grasping with tools: corticospinal excitability reflects observed hand movements. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  Mirroring avatars: dissociation of action and intention in human motor resonance , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  G. Csibra,et al.  Natural pedagogy , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Cosimo Urgesi,et al.  Corticospinal facilitation during first and third person imagery , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  P. Rossini,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: specificity of the effect and role of observer's orientation. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Modulation of premotor mirror neuron activity during observation of unpredictable grasping movements , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  S. Eckstein Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. , 2001, European journal of emergency medicine : official journal of the European Society for Emergency Medicine.

[30]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' understanding of object-directed action , 2005, Cognition.

[31]  F. Baldissera,et al.  Excitability changes in human corticospinal projections to muscles moving hand and fingers while viewing a reaching and grasping action , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[32]  T Brochier,et al.  A cortico-cortical mechanism mediating object-driven grasp in humans. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  Miguel Fernandez-del-Olmo,et al.  Movement observation specifies motor programs activated by the action observed objective , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.