The Effects of Prior Beliefs and Stimulus Prevalence on Self-Other Prioritization

Although self-relevance is widely acknowledged to enhance stimulus processing, the exclusivity of this effect remains open to question. In particular, in commonly adopted experimental paradigms, the prioritization of self-relevant (vs. other-relevant) material may reflect the operation of a taskspecific strategy rather than an obligatory facet of social-cognitive functioning. By changing basic aspects of the decisional context, it may therefore be possible to generate stimulus-prioritization effects for targets other than the self. Based on the demonstration that ownership facilitates object categorization (i.e., self-ownership effect), here we showed that stimulus prioritization is sensitive to prior expectations about the prevalence of forthcoming objects (owned-by-self vs. owned-byfriend) and whether these beliefs are supported during the task. Under conditions of stimulus uncertainty (i.e., no prior beliefs), replicating previous research, objects were classified more rapidly when owned-by-self compared to owned-by-friend (Expt. 1). When, however, the frequency of stimulus presentation either confirmed (Expt. 2) or disconfirmed (Expt. 3) prior expectations, stimulus prioritization was observed for the most prevalent objects regardless of their owner. A hierarchical drift diffusion model (HDDM) analysis further revealed that decisional bias was underpinned by differences in the evidential requirements of response generation. These findings underscore the flexibility of ownership effects (i.e., stimulus prioritization) during object processing.

[1]  Johanna K Falbén,et al.  Self-prioritization during stimulus processing is not obligatory , 2020, Psychological Research.

[2]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Parts of me: Identity-relevance moderates self-prioritization , 2019, Consciousness and Cognition.

[3]  P. Rotshtein,et al.  Self-prioritization and the attentional systems. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[4]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Valence and ownership: object desirability influences self-prioritization , 2019, Psychological Research.

[5]  G. Knoblich,et al.  Self-prioritization of fully unfamiliar stimuli , 2019, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  J. Pratt,et al.  I before U: Temporal order judgements reveal bias for self-owned objects , 2019, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  Marius Golubickis,et al.  How prioritized is self-prioritization during stimulus processing? , 2019, Visual Cognition.

[8]  Marco K. Wittmann,et al.  Neural mechanisms for learning self and other ownership , 2018, Nature Communications.

[9]  C. Frings,et al.  Searching for the inner self: evidence against a direct dependence of the self-prioritization effect on the ventro-medial prefrontal cortex , 2018, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Gregory L. Wade,et al.  Target self-relevance speeds visual search responses but does not improve search efficiency , 2018, Visual Cognition.

[11]  C. Macrae,et al.  Self-relevance enhances the benefits of attention on perception , 2018, Visual Cognition.

[12]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Mine or mother’s? Exploring the self-ownership effect across cultures , 2018, Culture and Brain.

[13]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The central locus of self-prioritisation , 2018, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  X. Gu,et al.  Self as Object: Emerging Trends in Self Research , 2017, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Exploring the Self-Ownership Effect: Separating Stimulus and Response Biases , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Self-prioritization and perceptual matching: The effects of temporal construal , 2017, Memory & Cognition.

[17]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  Can the Self Become Another? Investigating the Effects of Self-Association with a New Facial Identity , 2017, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The ubiquitous self: what the properties of self‐bias tell us about the self , 2017, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[19]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Self-Relevance Prioritizes Access to Visual Awareness , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  R. Chakravarthi,et al.  Does self-prioritization affect perceptual processes? , 2017 .

[21]  A. Seth,et al.  A social Bayesian brain: How social knowledge can shape visual perception , 2017, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  James M. Shine,et al.  Predictions penetrate perception: Converging insights from brain, behaviour and disorder , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN) , 2016, Cognitive neuroscience.

[24]  A. Kritikos,et al.  Culture modulates implicit ownership-induced self-bias in memory , 2016, Cognition.

[25]  Tom Verguts,et al.  Predictive information speeds up visual awareness in an individuation task by modulating threshold setting, not processing efficiency , 2016, Vision Research.

[26]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Wieske van Zoest,et al.  Testing the idea of privileged awareness of self-relevant information. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  D. Wentura,et al.  Self-Prioritization Beyond Perception. , 2015, Experimental psychology.

[29]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Integrative Self: How Self-Reference Integrates Perception and Memory , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  D. Wentura,et al.  About the composition of self-relevance: Conjunctions not features are bound to the self , 2015, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[31]  Matthew D. Weaver,et al.  Social salience does not transfer to oculomotor visual search , 2015 .

[32]  P. Rotshtein,et al.  Is it always me first? Effects of self-tagging on third-person perspective-taking. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Explanations of the endowment effect: an integrative review , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Jie Sui,et al.  The salient self: the left intraparietal sulcus responds to social as well as perceptual-salience after self-association. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  G. Humphreys,et al.  In-group modulation of perceptual matching , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[36]  Tom Beckers,et al.  A Bayesian hierarchical diffusion model decomposition of performance in Approach–Avoidance Tasks , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[37]  O. Lipp,et al.  Object ownership and action: the influence of social context and choice on the physical manipulation of personal property , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Joshua J. Tremel,et al.  Prior probability and feature predictability interactively bias perceptual decisions , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  D. Wentura,et al.  Self-priorization processes in action and perception. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian inferences about the self (and others): A review , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.

[41]  C. White,et al.  Decomposing bias in different types of simple decisions. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[42]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The automatic and the expected self: separating self- and familiarity biases effects by manipulating stimulus probability , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[43]  David J. Turk,et al.  Exploring early self-referential memory effects through ownership. , 2013, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[44]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  HDDM: Hierarchical Bayesian estimation of the Drift-Diffusion Model in Python , 2013, Front. Neuroinform..

[45]  Syaheed B. Jabar,et al.  The trajectory of the target probability effect , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[46]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Perceptual effects of social salience: evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  Gordon Pennycook,et al.  Reasoning with base rates is routine, relatively effortless, and context dependent , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[49]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Bias in the Brain: A Diffusion Model Analysis of Prior Probability and Potential Payoff , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  David J. Turk,et al.  Mine and Me: Exploring the Neural Basis of Object Ownership , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  A. Bayliss,et al.  Grasping the concept of personal property , 2011, Cognition.

[52]  M. Lee,et al.  Hierarchical diffusion models for two-choice response times. , 2011, Psychological methods.

[53]  Allen R McConnell,et al.  The Multiple Self-Aspects Framework: Self-Concept Representation and Its Implications , 2011, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[54]  J. Dreher,et al.  Decision Threshold Modulation in the Human Brain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  N. Brady,et al.  Self-face recognition is characterized by “bilateral gain” and by faster, more accurate performance which persists when faces are inverted , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[56]  N. Chater,et al.  The probabilistic mind: prospects for Bayesian cognitive science , 2008 .

[57]  C. Sedikides,et al.  Self-Enhancement: Food for Thought , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[58]  David J. Turk,et al.  Yours or mine? Ownership and memory , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[59]  Naomi M. Kenner,et al.  Low target prevalence is a stubborn source of errors in visual search tasks. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[60]  M. Bar The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  S. Sloman,et al.  Base-rate respect: From ecological rationality to dual processes. , 2007, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[62]  Hideki Ohira,et al.  An ERP study on self-relevant object recognition , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[63]  D. Hay Yours and mine: Toddlers' talk about possessions with familiar peers , 2006 .

[64]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  Thomas Gilovich,et al.  Are Adjustments Insufficient? , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[66]  J. L. Pierce,et al.  The State of Psychological Ownership: Integrating and Extending a Century of Research , 2003 .

[67]  M. Conway,et al.  The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. , 2000, Psychological review.

[68]  Lisa Maxfield,et al.  Attention and Semantic Priming: A Review of Prime Task Effects , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[69]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[70]  K. Shapiro,et al.  Personal names and the attentional blink: a visual "cocktail party" effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[71]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[72]  James K. Beggan On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. , 1992 .

[73]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status Quo Bias , 1991 .

[74]  P. Ramsey Possession Episodes in Young Children's Social Interactions , 1987 .

[75]  Felicia Pratto,et al.  Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection , 1986 .

[76]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[77]  Y. Trope,et al.  The effects of base rates and individuating information on judgments about another person , 1980 .

[78]  M. Bar-Hillel The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. , 1980 .

[79]  Lita Furby,et al.  The Origins and Early Development of Possessive Behavior , 1980 .

[80]  T. B. Rogers,et al.  Encoding of Personal Information: Self-Other Differences. , 1979 .

[81]  I. Ajzen Intuitive theories of events and the effects of base-rate information on prediction. , 1977 .

[82]  R. Todd,et al.  I Saw Mine First: A Prior-Entry Effect for Newly Acquired Ownership , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[83]  A. Voss,et al.  Diffusion Models in Experimental Psychology , 2013 .

[84]  Daphna Oyserman,et al.  Social Identity and Self-Regulation , 2007 .

[85]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction , 2004 .

[86]  N. G. Best,et al.  Bayesian deviance the e ective number of parameters and the comparison of arbitrarily complex models , 1998 .

[87]  Bharath Sriraman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases , 1974 .