Efficient Coding Theory Predicts a Tilt Aftereffect from Viewing Untilted Patterns

The brain is bombarded with a continuous stream of sensory information, but biological limitations on the data-transmission rate require this information to be encoded very efficiently [1]. Li and Atick [2] proposed that the two eyes' signals are coded efficiently in the brain using mutually decorrelated binocular summation and differencing channels; when a channel is strongly stimulated by the visual input, such that sensory noise is negligible, the channel should undergo temporary desensitization (known as adaptation). To date, the evidence for this theory has been limited [3, 4], and the binocular differencing channel is missing from many models of binocular integration [5-10]. Li and Atick's theory makes the remarkable prediction that perceived direction of tilt (clockwise or counterclockwise) of a test pattern can be controlled by pre-exposing observers to visual adaptation patterns that are untilted or even have no orientation signal. Here, we confirm this prediction. Each test pattern consisted of different images presented to the two eyes such that the binocular summation and difference signals were tilted in opposite directions, to give ambiguous information about tilt; by selectively desensitizing one or other of the binocular channels using untilted or non-oriented binocular adaptation patterns, we controlled the perceived tilt of the test pattern. Our results provide compelling evidence that the brain contains binocular summation and differencing channels that adapt to the prevailing binocular statistics.

[1]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Mechanisms of human motion perception revealed by a new cyclopean illusion. , 1986, Science.

[2]  C. Clifford,et al.  A functional angle on some after-effects in cortical vision , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[3]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Binocular interaction: contrast matching and contrast discrimination are predicted by the same model. , 2007, Spatial vision.

[4]  G. Poggio,et al.  Mechanisms of static and dynamic stereopsis in foveal cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  Frederick A A Kingdom,et al.  Dichoptic difference thresholds for uniform color changes applied to natural scenes. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[6]  David J. Heeger,et al.  A Model of Binocular Rivalry and Cross-orientation Suppression , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[7]  Andrew J. Parker,et al.  Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Jerome L. Myers,et al.  Research Design and Statistical Analysis , 1991 .

[9]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  Efficient stereo coding in the multiscale representation , 1994 .

[10]  Pat A. Walsh,et al.  Visualising the onset of meandering structures in a small length scale fluidic junction using dye injections , 2006, J. Vis..

[11]  George Sperling,et al.  A gain-control theory of binocular combination. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  G. Maehara,et al.  Binocular, Monocular and Dichoptic Pattern Masking , 2005 .

[13]  D. Snodderly,et al.  Organization of striate cortex of alert, trained monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): ongoing activity, stimulus selectivity, and widths of receptive field activating regions. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Her Mann Tsai,et al.  Effect of rounded edged dimple arrays on the boundary layer development , 2009, J. Vis..

[15]  Li Zhaoping,et al.  Gaze capture by eye-of-origin singletons: interdependence with awareness. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[16]  K. May,et al.  Perceived Direction of Motion Determined by Adaptation to Static Binocular Images , 2012, Current Biology.

[17]  T E Cohn,et al.  Binocular vision: two possible central interactions between signals from two eyes. , 1976, Science.

[18]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Dichoptic activation of the early motion system , 1993, Vision Research.

[19]  Sheng He,et al.  Orientation-selective adaptation and tilt after-effect from invisible patterns , 2001, Nature.

[20]  Li Zhaoping,et al.  Pre-attentive segmentation and correspondence in stereo. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[21]  Current Biology , 2012, Current Biology.

[22]  S. Engel,et al.  Neurons that detect interocular conflict during binocular rivalry revealed with EEG. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[23]  Erich W. Graf,et al.  Dichoptic motion perception limited to depth of fixation? , 2007, Vision Research.

[24]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[25]  Bruce G Cumming,et al.  Sensors for impossible stimuli may solve the stereo correspondence problem , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quantitative studies , 1937 .

[27]  Gian F. Poggio Mechanisms of Stereopsis in Monkey Visual Cortex , 1995 .

[28]  G. Poggio,et al.  Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  G C DeAngelis,et al.  The physiology of stereopsis. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[31]  Theodore E. Cohn,et al.  Binocular luminance detection: Availability of more than one central interaction , 1981, Vision Research.

[32]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  A psychophysical experiment to test the efficient stereo coding theory , 1998 .

[33]  F. Kingdom Binocular Vision: The Eyes Add and Subtract , 2012, Current Biology.

[34]  T. Meese,et al.  Binocular contrast interactions: Dichoptic masking is not a single process , 2007, Vision Research.

[35]  Masahiro Yamaguchi,et al.  Comparative Evaluation of Spectral Transforms for Multispectral Image Coding in Terms of Color Degradation , 2004 .

[36]  Frederick A.A. Kingdom,et al.  Dichoptic difference thresholds for chromatic stimuli , 2012, Vision Research.

[37]  Li Zhaoping,et al.  Understanding Vision: Theory, Models, and Data , 2014 .

[38]  T. Meese,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for two routes to suppression before binocular summation of signals in human vision , 2007, Neuroscience.

[39]  J. Gibson,et al.  ADAPTATION , AFTEREFFECT AND CONTRAST IN THE PERCEPTION OF TILTED LINES , 2004 .

[40]  A. Yuille,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Vision as Bayesian inference: analysis by synthesis? , 2022 .

[41]  D. Hubel,et al.  Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual cortex , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[42]  B. G. Cumming,et al.  Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception , 1997, Nature.

[43]  L. Helldin,et al.  Measuring adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder using the Quantified Behavior Test Plus , 2012, PsyCh journal.