Bimanual coordination during rhythmic movements in the absence of somatosensory feedback.

We investigated the role of somatosensory feedback during bimanual coordination by testing a bilaterally deafferented patient, a unilaterally deafferented patient, and three control participants on a repetitive bimanual circle-drawing task. Circles were drawn symmetrically or asymmetrically at varying speeds with full, partial, or no vision of the hands. Strong temporal coupling was observed between the hands at all movement rates during symmetrical drawing and at the comfortable movement rate during asymmetrical drawing in all participants. When making asymmetric movements at the comfortable and faster rates, the patients and controls exhibited similar evidence of pattern instability, including a reduction in temporal coupling and trajectory deformation. The patients differed from controls on measures of spatial coupling and variability. The amplitudes and shapes of the two circles were less similar across limbs for the patients than the controls and the circles produced by the patients tended to drift in extrinsic space across successive cycles. These results indicate that somatosensory feedback is not critical for achieving temporal coupling between the hands nor does it contribute significantly to the disruption of asymmetrical coordination at faster movement rates. However, spatial consistency and position, both within and between limbs, were disrupted in the absence of somatosensory feedback.

[1]  Richard B Ivry,et al.  Temporal Control and Coordination: The Multiple Timer Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  P Viviani,et al.  Cerebral dominance and asynchrony between bimanual two-dimensional movements. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Generation of Bimanual Trajectories of Disparate Eccentricity: Levels of Interference and Spontaneous Changes Over Practice , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Differential control of in-phase and anti-phase coupling of rhythmic movements of ipsilateral hand and foot , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Dynamic position sense during a cyclical drawing movement: effects of application and withdrawal of tendon vibration , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  R. Ivry,et al.  A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective on Bimanual Coordination and Interference , 2004 .

[8]  C. Bard,et al.  The role of proprioceptive information for the production of isometric forces and for handwriting tasks. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[9]  Elizabeth A Franz,et al.  Does Handedness Determine Which Hand Leads in a Bimanual Task? , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Rapid elbow flexion in the absence of proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[11]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[12]  J A Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. , 1988, Science.

[13]  C. Bard,et al.  Role of the feedforward command and reafferent information in the coordination of a passing prehension task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  C. Bard,et al.  Bimanual Interference in a Deafferented Patient and Control Subjects , 1994 .

[15]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  The role of vision in repetitive circle drawing. , 1996, Acta psychologica.

[16]  J. Summers,et al.  The Dynamics of Bimanual Circle Drawing , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[17]  C. Bard,et al.  Control of single-joint movements in deafferented patients: evidence for amplitude coding rather than position control , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  R. Ivry,et al.  When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  C. Bard,et al.  Contribution of proprioception for calibrating and updating the motor space. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[20]  R. Carson,et al.  Visual feedback alters the variations in corticospinal excitability that arise from rhythmic movements of the opposite limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Andras Semjen,et al.  Instabilities during antiphase bimanual movements: are ipsilateral pathways involved? , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  J. Sanes,et al.  Motor control in humans with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[23]  C. Bard,et al.  Deafferentation and pointing with visual double-step perturbations , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Roll Jp,et al.  Proprioceptive sensory codes mediating movement trajectory perception: human hand vibration-induced drawing illusions. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[25]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  The Cerebellum and Event Timing , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[26]  C. Ghez,et al.  Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  M Husain,et al.  The coordination of bimanual prehension movements in a centrally deafferented patient. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Direction information coordinated via the posterior third of the corpus callosum during bimanual movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Nicole Wenderoth,et al.  Parieto-premotor areas mediate directional interference during bimanual movements. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Order parameters for the neural organization of single, multijoint limb movement patterns , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  B. Heller Circular Statistics in Biology, Edward Batschelet. Academic Press, London & New York (1981), 371, Price $69.50 , 1983 .

[32]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[33]  Daniel Cattaert,et al.  Hand coordination in bimanual circle drawing. , 1995 .

[34]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: unimanual circle drawing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  J C Rothwell,et al.  Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  Giancarlo Ferrigno,et al.  Elite: A Digital Dedicated Hardware System for Movement Analysis Via Real-Time TV Signal Processing , 1985, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[37]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[38]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Disrupted Timing of Discontinuous But Not Continuous Movements by Cerebellar Lesions , 2003, Science.

[39]  A Semjen,et al.  Spontaneous and intentional pattern switching in a multisegmental bimanual coordination task. , 1999, Motor control.

[40]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Timing of bimanual movements and deafferentation: implications for the role of sensory movement effects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  H Heuer,et al.  Structural constraints on bimanual movements , 1993, Psychological research.

[42]  J. Roll,et al.  Proprioceptive sensory codes mediating movement trajectory perception: human hand vibration-induced drawing illusions. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[43]  S. P. Swinnen Coordination of upper-limb movement : a neuro-dynamics account , 1992 .

[44]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Systematic error in the organization of physical action , 2001, Cogn. Sci..

[45]  L. Cohen,et al.  Synchronous Bimanual Movements Performed by Homologous and Non-Homologous Muscles , 1971, Perceptual and motor skills.

[46]  R. Ivry,et al.  Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Between-limb asynchronies during bimanual coordination: Effects of manual dominance and attentional cueing , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Jonathan Cole,et al.  Living without touch and peripheral information about body position and movement: Studies with deafferented subjects. , 1995 .

[49]  J. R. Bloedel,et al.  Adaptation of reach-to-grasp movement in response to force perturbations , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  Daniel Cattaert,et al.  Simulating a neural cross-talk model for between-hand interference during bimanual circle drawing , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[51]  C Ghez,et al.  Proprioceptive control of interjoint coordination. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[52]  A. Daffertshofer,et al.  Modeling Rhythmic Interlimb Coordination: Beyond the Haken–Kelso–Bunz Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: bimanual circle drawing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  M. Billon,et al.  The role of sensory information in the production of periodic finger-tapping sequences , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  Spatial topological constraints in a bimanual task. , 1991, Acta psychologica.