Modeling the age-related associative deficit in self-initiated recall

We use Howard & Kahana’s (2002) Temporal Context Model (TCM) to help explain the well-known age-related deficit in episodic recall. TCM is a distributed memory model that postulates two basic forms of information: item and context. Throughout both study and recall, TCM associates the presently active item with a distributed representation of context that evolves over time. The model postulates that retrieval of contextual states that were associated with the currently active item adds to the current state of context. Using this retrieved context as a cue activates nearby items, producing the well-documented forward-biased associative tendencies seen in recall. Likewise, using time-of-test context as a cue activates recently experienced items, producing the recency effect in recall. Fitting TCM to data on episodic recall by young and older adults, we show that associative deficits in older adults can be understood in terms of a failure to inhibit noise associated with contextual retrieval. Not all forms of memory are equally impaired over the normal course of human aging. Whereas older adults invariably show significant impairment on episodic memory tasks, such as recognition or recall, their performance on implicit memory tasks, such as perceptual priming, is only slightly impaired (La Voie & Light, 1992; Verhaeghen, Marcoen, & Goossens, 1993). Among episodic memory tasks, older adults have the greatest difficulty with recall tasks, especially those that provide minimal contextual support, such as associative recall of unrelated words (NavehBenjamin, 2000), or free recall of randomly arranged word lists (Ceci & Tabor, 1981; Craik, Byrd, & Swanson, 1987; Laurence, 1967; Kahana, Howard, Zaromb, & Wingfield, 2002; Kahana & Wingfield, 2000). This paper uses mathematical modeling techniques to help clarify the age-related deficit in episodic recall. We focus on the age-related deficit in free recall due to recent advances in our understanding of the component processes underlying performance of this task (Kahana, 1996; Howard This research was funded by National Institutes of Health grants AG15852 and MH55687. We are also grateful for support from the W. M. Keck foundation. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael J. Kahana, Volen National Center for Complex Systems, MS 013, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 02454-9110. Marc Howard is now at Boston University, Department of Psychology, 64 Cummington Street, Boston, MA 02215-2407. Electronic mail may be sent to kahana@brandeis.edu. TEMPORAL CONTEXT MODEL 2 & Kahana, 1999, 2001; Kahana et al., 2002). Section 1 of this paper describes how the retrieval dynamics in free recall, as measured by output order effects, can be used to separate associative and recency-sensitive retrieval processes. Section 2 describes the Temporal Context Model (TCM) of free recall, and shows how it accounts for recency and contiguity effects in free recall. Section 3 demonstrates that TCM can account for the intact recency effects and impaired contiguity effects observed in free recall with older participants. Section 4 discusses the implications of this modeling work for theories of cognitive aging. 1. Recency, association, and the decomposition of free recall in normal aging In the free recall task, participants are asked to recall as many items as they can from a recently presented list, in any order. Going beyond a simple measure of recall accuracy, one can plot the probability of recalling items as a function of their serial position within the list. The characteristic feature of the serial position curve, which has fueled much of the theory on retrieval from episodic memory, is the presence of a small primacy and a large recency effect. The primacy effect, a response preference for the first few items of a list, is much smaller than the recency effect and depends critically on rehearsal (Brodie & Murdock, 1977; Rundus, 1971; Tan & Ward, 2000). The recency effect was, until recently, seen as evidence for the operations of a phonological buffer, capable of rehearsing and maintaining a small number of items in the absence of interfering activity (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1981). However, the finding that the recency effect persists over long time scales (Bjork & Whitten, 1974; Glenberg et al., 1980; Howard & Kahana, 1999) has led a growing number of researchers to ascribe the recency effect to positional distinctiveness (Nairne, Neath, Serra, & Byun, 1997; Neath & Crowder, 1990) or temporal context (Howard & Kahana, 2002). The serial position curve, which has played a key role in almost all theories of human memory function (Murdock, 1960; Anderson, 1976; Metcalfe & Murdock, 1981; Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980, 1981; Anderson, Bothell, Lebiere, & Matessa, 1998), reflects the end product of a complex and dynamic process. A weakness in traditional serial position analyses is in the way they have focused on the probability of recall as a function of presentation order while disregarding the order of recall. As such, they have discarded information about sequential dependencies in retrieval. Such information, we argue, is crucial for understanding the process of self-initiated memory retrieval. Figure 1a shows serial position curves for young and older participants performing an immediate free recall task (as reported in Experiment 1 of Kahana et al., 2002). Figure 1d shows serial position curves for delayed free recall (Experiment 2 of Kahana et al., 2002). The age-related deficit in free recall is generally seen at all serial positions, though it becomes somewhat less pronounced towards the end of the serial position curve (Parkinson, Lindholm, & Inman, 1982; Foos, Sabol, Corral, & Mobley, 1987; Rissenberg & Glanzer, 1987; Poitrenaud, Moy, Girousse, Wolmark, & Piette, 1989; Capitani, Della Sala, Logie, & Spinnler, 1992; Kahana et al., 2002). As expected, the recency effect seen in immediate free recall is largely attenuated by a 16 s arithmetic distractor task given to participants at the end of the list in the delayed experiment. Howard and Kahana (1999) proposed a decomposition of free recall performance into two measures: 1) a measure of how participants initiate recall, and 2) a measure of the way participants make transitions in recall from one item to the next. Combined with a stopping rule, these two measures summarize the dynamics of retrieval, and can predict the form of the serial position curve. TEMPORAL CONTEXT MODEL 3 2 4 6 8 10 Serial Position 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 P ro ba bi lit y of R ec al l a.

[1]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..

[2]  Michael J Kahana,et al.  Age dissociates recency and lag recency effects in free recall. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[3]  L. Hasher,et al.  Cognitive gerontology and attentional inhibition: a reply to Burke and McDowd. , 1997, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[4]  Daniel S. Rizzuto,et al.  An Autoassociative Neural Network Model of Paired-Associate Learning , 2001, Neural Computation.

[5]  Marc W Howard,et al.  Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  Mark A. Sabol,et al.  Age differences in primary and secondary memory , 1987 .

[7]  B. Murdock,et al.  The distinctiveness of stimuli. , 1960, Psychological review.

[8]  Richard M. Shiffrin,et al.  SAM: A theory of probabilistic search in associative memory. , 1980 .

[9]  M. Naveh-Benjamin Adult age differences in memory performance: tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  A. Wingfield,et al.  A functional relation between learning and organization in free recall , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Recency, Primacy, and Memory: Reappraising and Standardising the Serial Position Curve , 1992, Cortex.

[12]  John R. Anderson Language, Memory, and Thought , 1976 .

[13]  J. S. Nairne,et al.  Positional Distinctiveness and the Ratio Rule in Free Recall , 1997 .

[14]  M. Bradley,et al.  A two-process account of long-term serial position effects , 1980 .

[15]  V W Inman,et al.  An analysis of age differences in immediate recall. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[16]  D. A. Brodie,et al.  Effect of Presentation Time on Nominal and Functional Serial-Position Curves of Free Recall. , 1977 .

[17]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  Recency effect in anterograde amnesia: Evidence for distinct memory stores underlying enhanced retrieval of terminal items in immediate and delayed recall paradigms , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  M. Laurence,et al.  Memory loss with age: A test of two strategies for its retardation , 1967 .

[19]  S. Ceci,et al.  Flexibility and memory: are the elderly really less flexible? , 1981, Experimental aging research.

[20]  M. Kahana Associative symmetry and memory theory , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[21]  D. Rundus Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. , 1971 .

[22]  G. Ward,et al.  A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  L L Light,et al.  Adult age differences in repetition priming: a meta-analysis. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[24]  M. Glanzer,et al.  Free recall and word finding ability in normal aging and senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type: the effect of item concreteness. , 1987, Journal of gerontology.

[25]  Marc W Howard,et al.  When Does Semantic Similarity Help Episodic Retrieval , 2002 .

[26]  B. Murdock Context and mediators in a theory of distributed associative memory (TODAM2). , 1997 .

[27]  C. Lebiere,et al.  An integrated theory of list memory. , 1998 .

[28]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.

[29]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  Mark Byrd,et al.  Patterns of memory loss in three elderly samples. , 1987, Psychology and aging.

[31]  L. Goossens,et al.  Facts and fiction about memory aging: a quantitative integration of research findings. , 1993, Journal of gerontology.

[32]  R. Bjork,et al.  Recency-sensitive retrieval processes in long-term free recall☆ , 1974 .

[33]  M. Kahana Associative retrieval processes in free recall , 1996, Memory & cognition.

[34]  Janet Metcalfe,et al.  An encoding and retrieval model of single-trial free recall , 1981 .

[35]  R. M. Hogan Interitem encoding and directed search in free recall , 1975, Memory & cognition.

[36]  D. Yntema,et al.  Recall as a search process , 1963 .

[37]  W. Estes Statistical theory of distributional phenomena in learning. , 1955, Psychological review.

[38]  W. Estes Statistical theory of spontaneous recovery and regression. , 1955, Psychological review.

[39]  E. Raskin,et al.  The strength and direction of associations formed in the learning of nonsense syllables , 1937 .

[40]  D. Laming Testing the Idea of Distinct Storage Mechanisms in Memory , 1999 .

[41]  Marc W. Howard,et al.  A distributed representation of temporal context , 2002 .

[42]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  A model for interference and forgetting , 1988 .

[43]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Search of associative memory. , 1981 .

[44]  R. G. Crowder,et al.  Schedules of presentation and temporal distinctiveness in human memory. , 1990 .

[45]  M. Kahana,et al.  Associative asymmetry in probed recall of serial lists , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[46]  M J Kahana,et al.  Adult age differences in the temporal characteristics of category free recall. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[47]  J. Deese,et al.  Serial effects in recall of unorganized and sequentially organized verbal material. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[48]  F Moy,et al.  Psychometric procedures for analysis of memory losses in the elderly. , 1989, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics. Supplement.

[49]  Murray Glanzer,et al.  Storage Mechanisms in Recall , 1972 .