In search of vigilance: the problem of iatrogenically created psychological phenomena.

To what extent are identified psychological processes created in laboratories? The present work addresses this issue with reference to one particular realm of behavior: vigilance. Specifically, I argue that the classic vigilance decrement function can be viewed more realistically and advantageously as an "invigilant" increment function. Rather than characterizing the transient decrease in detection capability that is evident on exposure to enforced monitoring as a diminishment in capacity, it may be more usefully seen as an appropriate scaling by the designated observer to adapt to the nonoptimal circumstances that he or she is forced to endure. This proposition emphasizes the dynamic response characteristics of the observer and locates the origin of the phenomenon and the onus for practical improvements in the design of operational displays with designers rather than apportioning blame for performance decrements to the operator. This perspective reinforces the recognition of a crucial presence of the necessary but often unrecognized external arbiter in the vigilance paradigm and the extrinsically imposed imperative to sustain attention. Explicit recognition of this fact also helps explain the stress involved with extended vigils. In identifying the traditional vigilance decrement as a form of iatrogenic disease, I argue that modern design of work systems should alleviate the need for either the acute or the chronic expressions of such enforced human monitoring activity. It is possible that the case of vigilance is itself representative of a modern propensity to create new psychological phenomena in the face of human exposure to modern, evolving technical environments.

[1]  N. Mackworth The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search 1 , 1948 .

[2]  J. Swets,et al.  A decision-making theory of visual detection. , 1954, Psychological review.

[3]  B. Bergum,et al.  END-SPURT IN VIGILANCE. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  J. Mcgrath,et al.  Vigilance: a symposium , 1963 .

[5]  L. Buck Reaction time as a measure of perceptual vigilance. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  J. Brady,et al.  Signal frequency and shock probability as determinants of prolonged vigilance performance in rhesus monkeys. , 1972, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[7]  C. G. Drury,et al.  An Industrial Study of the Effects of Feedback and Fault Density on Inspection Performance , 1973 .

[8]  W H Teichner,et al.  The Detection of a Simple Visual Signal as a Function of Time of Watch , 1974, Human factors.

[9]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[10]  Angus Craig,et al.  Is the Vigilance Decrement Simply a Response Adjustment Towards Probability Matching? , 1978 .

[11]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[12]  Colin G. Drury,et al.  Training Programs for Inspection , 1981 .

[13]  R Parasuraman,et al.  Visual sustained attention: image degradation produces rapid sensitivity decrement over time. , 1983, Science.

[14]  J. Warm Sustained attention in human performance , 1984 .

[15]  David R. Williams,et al.  Job Stress and Blue Collar Work , 1987 .

[16]  A Craig,et al.  Signal Detection Theory and Probability Matching Apply to Vigilance , 1985, Human factors.

[17]  D. Holding,et al.  Limitations of the Cognitive Vigilance Increment , 1987, Human factors.

[18]  C. Sweeley,et al.  Interactive and multi-sensory analysis of complex mixtures by an automated gas chromatography system. , 1987, Journal of chromatography.

[19]  E L Wiener,et al.  Application of Vigilance Research: Rare, Medium, or Well Done? , 1987, Human factors.

[20]  D. Broadbent,et al.  Work Preferences as Moderators of the Effects of Paced and Unpaced Work on Mood and Cognitive Performance: A Laboratory Simulation of Mechanized Letter Sorting , 1990, Human factors.

[21]  K. J. Vicente,et al.  The Ecology of Human-Machine Systems II: Mediating 'Direct Perception' in Complex Work Domains , 1990 .

[22]  Kim J. Vicente,et al.  Ecological interface design: theoretical foundations , 1992, IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern..

[23]  P. Hancock,et al.  Experimental Evaluation of a Model of Mental Workload , 1993 .

[24]  David A. Sawin,et al.  The Effects of Subject-Controlled Pacing and Task Type on Sustained Attention and Subjective Workload , 1993 .

[25]  Mark W. Scerbo,et al.  Effects of Instruction Type and Boredom Proneness in Vigilance: Implications for Boredom and Workload , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[26]  P A Hancock,et al.  Influence of task demand characteristics on workload and performance. , 1995, The International journal of aviation psychology.

[27]  G. R. J. Hockey Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[28]  Peter A. Hancock,et al.  On the Future of Work , 1997 .

[29]  M. Scerbo What's so boring about vigilance? , 1998 .

[30]  E. Deci,et al.  Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[31]  M. Scerbo STRESS, WORKLOAD, AND BOREDOM IN VIGILANCE: A PROBLEM AND AN ANSWER. , 2001 .

[32]  Kim J. Vicente,et al.  Ecological Interface Design: Progress and Challenges , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[33]  J. Arruda,et al.  Rhythmic Oscillations in the Performance of a Sustained Attention Task , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[34]  Todd D. Hollander,et al.  Signal regularity and the mindlessness model of vigilance. , 2005, British journal of psychology.

[35]  Peter A. Hancock,et al.  Hedonomics: The Power of Positive and Pleasurable Ergonomics , 2005 .

[36]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  26.5 brief comms NEW , 2005 .

[37]  W. T. Nelson,et al.  Target Acquisition With UAVs: Vigilance Displays and Advanced Cuing Interfaces , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[38]  W. N. Dember,et al.  Training for vigilance: the effect of knowledge of results format and dispositional optimism and pessimism on performance and stress. , 2006, British journal of psychology.

[39]  Tyler H. Shaw,et al.  Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[40]  Joshua Foer,et al.  Moonwalking with Einstein: The Art and Science of Remembering Everything , 2011 .

[41]  J. Szalma Workload and stress in vigilance: the impact of display format and task type. , 2011, The American journal of psychology.

[42]  Sidney Dekker,et al.  Drift into Failure: From Hunting Broken Components to Understanding Complex Systems , 2011 .

[43]  Simon Farrell,et al.  Abstract Concepts Require Concrete Models: Why Cognitive Scientists Have Not Yet Embraced Nonlinearly Coupled, Dynamical, Self-Organized Critical, Synergistic, Scale-Free, Exquisitely Context-Sensitive, Interaction-Dominant, Multifractal, Interdependent Brain-Body-Niche Systems , 2012, Top. Cogn. Sci..